Editorial

Our Vision Is Your Opportunity

Adrian Brügger1, Isabel Richter*2

Global Environmental Psychology, 2023, Vol. 1, Article e11841, https://doi.org/10.5964/gep.11841

Published (VoR): 2023-11-06.

*Corresponding author at: Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social and Educational Sciences, Bygg 12, Dragvoll, Norway. E-mail: isabel.richter@ntnu.no

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Click to enlarge
gep.11841-f1
Figure 1

Key Elements of GEP’s Vision

Sharing insights is a fundamental part of science. For many disciplines, academic journals are the key channel through which researchers share what they have learnt through their empirical and theoretical work. However, the existing academic publishing system has some serious flaws. In this editorial, we briefly point to some of these flaws and describe what steps we have already taken and are planning to take to make Global Environmental Psychology (GEP) a journal that will help to make research in our field more accessible, diverse and transparent (GEP’s key priorities are summarized in Figure 1).

Accessibility

First, we would like to raise the issue of inequitable access. A lot of research is published behind a paywall, in subscription-based journals, meaning that readers can only access publications if their institutions have a costly subscription or if they pay for the individual article. More recently, the problem of accessibility has been somewhat mitigated by business models that make articles freely available but instead charge authors an often immense publication fee. While this alternative business model is better for readers, the publication fees create a new barrier for authors and their institutions, putting those with fewer resources at a disadvantage (Ross-Hellauer et al., 2022). At GEP, there are no cost-related access barriers, neither for authors nor for readers. Thanks to public funding made available through the Leibniz Institute for Psychology (ZPID) based in Trier (Germany) all publications are published and accessible free of charge.

Accessibility can also be hampered by linguistic barriers. The use of jargon, highly technical method and result sections, and the prevalent use of English makes academic work difficult to digest for readers with other professional, educational, and linguistic backgrounds. To facilitate understanding for a broad readership and dissemination, GEP asks authors to prepare a non-technical summary for a non-academic audience. Moreover, authors can add a non-English abstract to their work.

Diversity

In addition to accessibility problems, environmental psychology struggles with a lack of diversity. A large part of the research published in the leading journals in the field of environmental psychology relies on north-western Europe and British-descent samples, hence a very small and by no means representative proportion of humanity (Tam & Milfont, 2020). The lack of insights from currently underrepresented groups prevents not only the diversification of knowledge, but also the validation of psychological theories across human groups (Henrich et al., 2010).

To contribute to more diverse research in environmental psychology, GEP values diversity as a key criterion when evaluating manuscripts (see our Review Guidelines). We consider theoretical, methodological (e.g., quantitative, qualitative, mixed methods), and sociocultural diversity. Authors are explicitly encouraged to diversify their work (e.g., by collaborating with underrepresented co-authors or collecting data with underrepresented samples) and to discuss the generalizability of their findings.

Click to enlarge
gep.11841-f2
Figure 2

Diversity Badge for Medium (A) and Small Print Areas (B)

As a particularly innovative step, GEP awards a diversity badge if articles declare what steps they have taken to increase diversity (Figure 2; Brügger & Richter, 2023). The purpose of this diversity statement (for an example, see end of this article; see also Cell Press, 2023) is twofold. First, it makes diversity issues and possible countermeasures visible for authors and readers. Second, similar to Open Science Badges (Blohowiak et al., 2013), the diversity badge should work as an incentive for authors. Our hope is that this encourages the field to collectively move towards a more diverse academic landscape.

On a structural level, GEP embraces diversity within the journal’s editorial team and board members. About half of our editors and Editorial Board members are from geographically diverse backgrounds (i.e., from countries outside North America and Northwestern Europe; Henrich et al., 2010; Steltenpohl et al., 2021). This diverse team facilitates global outreach and publication of work coming from otherwise underrepresented regions and populations.

Open and Transparent Research

In the last two decades, standard research practices have increasingly come under scrutiny because they undermine key principles of good science such as transparency and reproducibility (Open Science Collaboration, 2015); while some practices are “merely” questionable, others are simply fraud (Spellman, 2015). Fortunately, there are promising steps that authors and publishers can take to mitigate these problems and improve the quality of research (e.g., Artner et al., 2021; Chambers & Tzavella, 2022).

Transparent, reproducible, and open research practices are paramount to GEP. We have therefore adopted ambitious open science standards (Aczel et al., 2020; Nosek et al., 2015). These include, for example, the requirement that authors share their data, materials, and code. Moreover, GEP encourages authors to submit their work as Registered Reports, where the methods and planned analyses are reviewed prior to data collection. If the quality of the submission is sufficiently high, the article is provisionally accepted for publication before data collection starts (Chambers & Tzavella, 2022). Another measure to increase transparency is that GEP makes pre-publication peer-reviews available alongside published articles. We are proud that, thanks to our ambitious open science policies, we are currently in 5th place among the 2,613 ranked journals in the TOP factor ranking (as of 23 June 2023; see https://topfactor.org/journals).

Evidently, implementing these standards comes at several (non-financial) costs: Authors must adhere to reporting guidelines when writing their manuscript, properly document and share their data, materials, and code, and answer extra questions during the submission process. Thus, making research more open requires skills that some authors need to learn. Fortunately, there are excellent tutorials and introductions into open science (Crüwell et al., 2019; Heise et al., 2023; Kathawalla et al., 2021), some of which are included in the official GEP article template. We plan to provide further resources in the future.

GEP’s open science policies do not only create extra work for authors, but also for editors: They need to ensure that authors adhere to the guidelines and, together with the publishing team, check the eligibility for open science badges and make relevant information about adherence to the standards publicly available (e.g., the transparency checklist to enable external evaluation of concordance between policies and procedures; Mayo-Wilson et al., 2021).

Although the submission process at GEP is more taxing compared to journals with laxer open science standards, we are convinced that open and transparent research is the only way forward and that it will pay off in the long run. Transparency and openness are crucial ingredients to research quality and increase trust in research outcomes (Schneider et al., 2022; Song et al., 2022). Individual researchers benefit from adopting open science practices through reputational gains, increased chances of publication, and more citations (Allen & Mehler, 2019; McKiernan et al., 2016; Ottaviani, 2016).

Your Opportunity

We are excited to offer GEP as a platform for accessible, diverse, and transparent research. But to make this vision a reality, we need all of you. Only if we work together, can we make a change.

You can contribute in various ways and roles. As an author you can contribute to implementing our vision by submitting your best work. How progressively you want to implement open science and diversity standards is up to you. You can either do just the minimum and adhere to our open science and diversity standards. Or you raise the bar by pre-registering your study or submitting it as a Registered Report. A progressive step to promote diversity could be to determine the order of authors based on the level of marginalization that they are experiencing in the academic and the wider societal context (the social justice based Academic Wheel of Privilege may be useful to this end, Elsherif et al., 2022).

In addition to submitting your own work, you can support GEP’s vision as a reviewer and use diversity and transparency as criteria when assessing the work of peers (see GEP’s manuscript evaluation criteria).

Opportunities to contribute to GEP’s vision will also come up outside of publication-oriented activities. We are planning capacity building measures intended mainly for researchers from underprivileged backgrounds and those early in their careers. One way to contribute could be to share and evaluate resources to promote research and writing skills (e.g., open science tutorials, programming courses, academic writing tools).

We are also exploring the idea of introducing an academic matchmaking platform. This platform would allow researchers to search for and collaborate with peers who have complementing skills (e.g., specific methodological, analytical, cultural, linguistic expertise) or resources (e.g., access to specific populations, financial resources, computing power). Facilitating collaboration in this way will build capacity for all researchers involved. You can contribute to this project by joining the planned matchmaking platform and offering your expertise to those who would profit the most. As an editor or reviewer, you could direct authors of submissions that have potential but also severe weaknesses to the platform to seek support in improving their work. If you are interested in the latter three opportunities, watch out for calls for assistance on the journal’s website and on social media, read future editorials, or get in touch with us directly.

Obviously you can also become an advocate of open science and diversity in ways that are independent of GEP. You can more generally adopt open science practices in your work and support your colleagues, students, or supervisor in so doing (for tips see Crüwell et al., 2019; Heise et al., 2023; Kathawalla et al., 2021). When you submit your work for publication, choose an outlet with high transparency and openness standards (see https://topfactor.org/). To promote diversity, you may reach out to under-privileged academics for collaboration and give them priority in the order of authors or offer them the role as PI or co-PI in fellow applications. As a student, you could critically reflect on the diversity aspect in your learning materials. If you are writing an essay or a thesis, you and your supervisor could actively look for diverse sources to base your work on (for additional ideas to diversify psychological research, see Puthillam et al., 2022; Rad et al., 2018; Steltenpohl et al., 2021; Tam & Milfont, 2020).

For the future of GEP, we are looking forward to reading, administering, and publishing high quality work that signals that our field is changing: becoming more accessible, more diverse, and more transparent.

Openness and Transparency Statements

The present article has been checked by its handling editor(s) for compliance with the journal's open science and transparency policies. The completed Transparency Checklist is publicly available at: https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.13194

Author Contributions

Adrian Brügger: Conceptualization | Writing – original draft | Writing – review & editing.

Isabel Richter: Writing – original draft | Writing – review & editing.

Acknowledgments

The authors want to use this editorial to acknowledge and thank the people without whom this journal would never have seen the light of day. Sabine Pahl, Wouter Poortinga, Linda Steg, and Claude Messner, thank you for believing in us and supporting us motivationally, intellectually, and institutionally in founding GEP. Janet Veldstra, thank you for contributing so actively during the brainstorming and application stage. Armin Günther and PsychOpen team, thank you for giving us the chance to develop this journal and for sharing your vast experience, critical thinking and constructive advice. Leah Rangi, thank you for designing this beautiful diversity badge for us. Raphael Krastev, thank you for helping design the journal’s logo.

Funding

The authors have no funding to report.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Diversity

In the list below, the check mark () indicates which steps were taken to increase diversity within the context of this paper. Steps that were not taken or did not apply are unmarked ().

Ethnically or otherwise diverse sample(s)

Gender balanced sample(s)

Inclusive gender measure

Inclusive materials

Sampling justification

Extensive sample description

Discussion of generalizability

Diverse reference list

Underprivileged / minority author(s)

Early career author(s)

Degree of privilege/marginalization considered in authorship order

Author(s) from sampled population (avoiding ’helicopter science’)

Supplementary Materials

The following table provides an overview of the accessibility of supplementary materials (if any) for this paper.

Type of supplementary materials Availability/Access
Data Not applicable (no data analysed).
Code Not applicable (no analysis code used).
Material Not applicable (no materials used).
Study/Analysis preregistration Not applicable (not an empirical study).

Openess and Transparency Badges

Open data: NO.

Open code: NO.

Open materials: NO.

Preregistration: NO.

Diversity statement: YES.

Note: YES = the present article meets the criteria for awarding the badge. NO = the present article does not meet the criteria for awarding the badge or the criteria are not applicable.

References

  • Aczel, B., Szaszi, B., Sarafoglou, A., Kekecs, Z., Kucharský, Š., Benjamin, D., Chambers, C. D., Fisher, A., Gelman, A., Gernsbacher, M. A., Ioannidis, J. P., Johnson, E., Jonas, K., Kousta, S., Lilienfeld, S. O., Lindsay, D. S., Morey, C. C., Munafò, M., Newell, B. R., …Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2020). A consensus-based transparency checklist. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(1), 4-6. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0772-6

  • Allen, C., & Mehler, D. M. A. (2019). Open science challenges, benefits and tips in early career and beyond. PLoS Biology, 17(5), Article e3000246. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000246

  • Artner, R., Verliefde, T., Steegen, S., Gomes, S., Traets, F., Tuerlinckx, F., & Vanpaemel, W. (2021). The reproducibility of statistical results in psychological research: An investigation using unpublished raw data. Psychological Methods, 26(5), 527-546. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000365

  • Blohowiak, B. B., Cohoon, J., de-Wit, L., Eich, E., Farach, F. J., Hasselman, F., Holcombe, A. O., Humphreys, M., Lewis, M., & Nosek, B. A. (2013). Badges to acknowledge open practices. OSF. https://osf.io/tvyxz/

  • Brügger, A., & Richter, I. (2023). Diversity badge. OSF. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/5FTK7

  • Cell Press. (2023). Inclusion and diversity statement–FAQs. Cell Press. https://www.cell.com/inclusion-diversity-statement-faqs

  • Chambers, C. D., & Tzavella, L. (2022). The past, present and future of Registered Reports. Nature Human Behaviour, 6, 29-42. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01193-7

  • Crüwell, S., van Doorn, J., Etz, A., Makel, M. C., Moshontz, H., Niebaum, J. C., Orben, A., Parsons, S., & Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M. (2019). Seven easy steps to open science. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 227(4), 237-248. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000387

  • Elsherif, M., Middleton, S., Phan, J. M., Azevedo, F., Iley, B., Grose-Hodge, M., Tyler, S., Kapp, S., Gourdon-Kanhukamwe, A., Grafton-Clarke, D., Yeung, S. K., Shaw, J. J., Hartmann, H., & Dokovova, M. (2022). Bridging neurodiversity and open scholarship: How shared values can guide best practices for research integrity, social justice, and principled education. MetaArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31222/osf.io/k7a9p

  • Heise, V., Holman, C., Lo, H., Lyras, E. M., Adkins, M. C., Aquino, M. R. J., Bougioukas, K. I., Bray, K. O., Gajos, M., Guo, X., Hartling, C., Huerta-Gutierrez, R., Jindrová, M., Kenney, J. P. M., Kępińska, A. P., Kneller, L., Lopez-Rodriguez, E., Mühlensiepen, F., Richards, A., …Weissgerber, T. L. (2023). Ten simple rules for implementing open and reproducible research practices after attending a training course. PLoS Computational Biology, 19(1), Article e1010750. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010750

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61-83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

  • Kathawalla, U.-K., Silverstein, P., & Syed, M. (2021). Easing into open science: A guide for graduate students and their advisors. Collabra: Psychology, 7(1), Article 18684. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.18684

  • Mayo-Wilson, E., Grant, S., Supplee, L., Kianersi, S., Amin, A., DeHaven, A., & Mellor, D. (2021). Evaluating implementation of the Transparency and Openness Promotion (TOP) guidelines: The TRUST process for rating journal policies, procedures, and practices. Research Integrity and Peer Review, 6(1), Article 9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41073-021-00112-8

  • McKiernan, E. C., Bourne, P. E., Brown, C. T., Buck, S., Kenall, A., Lin, J., McDougall, D., Nosek, B. A., Ram, K., Soderberg, C. K., Spies, J. R., Thaney, K., Updegrove, A., Woo, K. H., & Yarkoni, T. (2016). How open science helps researchers succeed. ELife, 5, Article e16800. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16800

  • Nosek, B. A., Alter, G., Banks, G. C., Borsboom, D., Bowman, S. D., Breckler, S. J., Buck, S., Chambers, C. D., Chin, G., Christensen, G., Contestabile, M., Dafoe, A., Eich, E., Freese, J., Glennerster, R., Goroff, D., Green, D. P., Hesse, B., Humphreys, M., …Yarkoni, T. (2015). Promoting an open research culture. Science, 348(6242), 1422-1425. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aab2374

  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), Article aac4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716

  • Ottaviani, J. (2016). The post-embargo open access citation advantage: It exists (probably), it’s modest (usually), and the rich get richer (of course). PLOS ONE, 11(8), Article e0159614. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159614

  • Puthillam, A., Doble, L. J. M., Santos, J. J. D., Elsherif, M., Steltenpohl, C. N., Moreau, D., Pownall, M., & Kapoor, H. (2022). Guidelines to improve internationalization in psychological science. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2u4h5

  • Rad, M. S., Martingano, A. J., & Ginges, J. (2018). Toward a psychology of Homo sapiens: Making psychological science more representative of the human population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(45), 11401-11405. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721165115

  • Ross-Hellauer, T., Reichmann, S., Cole, N. L., Fessl, A., Klebel, T., & Pontika, N. (2022). Dynamics of cumulative advantage and threats to equity in open science: A scoping review. Royal Society Open Science, 9(1), Article 211032. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.211032

  • Schneider, J., Rosman, T., Kelava, A., & Merk, S. (2022). Do open-science badges increase trust in scientists among undergraduates, scientists, and the public? Psychological Science, 33(9), 1588-1604. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221097499

  • Song, H., Markowitz, D. M., & Taylor, S. H. (2022). Trusting on the shoulders of open giants? Open science increases trust in science for the public and academics. Journal of Communication, 72(4), 497-510. https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqac017

  • Spellman, B. A. (2015). A short (personal) future history of Revolution 2.0. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(6), 886-899. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615609918

  • Steltenpohl, C. N., Montilla Doble, L. J., Basnight-Brown, D. M., Dutra, N. B., Belaus, A., Kung, C.-C., Onie, S., Seernani, D., Chen, S.-C., Burin, D. I., & Darda, K. (2021). Society for the Improvement of Psychological Science Global Engagement Task Force Report. Collabra: Psychology, 7(1), Article 22968. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.22968

  • Tam, K.-P., & Milfont, T. L. (2020). Towards cross-cultural environmental psychology: A state-of-the-art review and recommendations. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 71, Article 101474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101474