Research Articles

Measuring the Effectiveness of Value-Framing and Message Valence on Audience Engagement Across Countries

Katie Blake*1, Takahiro Kubo1,2, Diogo Veríssimo1

Global Environmental Psychology, 2023, Vol. 1, Article e11181, https://doi.org/10.5964/gep.11181

Received: 2023-01-22. Accepted: 2023-07-05. Published (VoR): 2023-11-06.

Handling Editor: Esther Katharina Papies, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, Scotland, United Kingdom

*Corresponding author at: Department of Biology, 11a Mansfield Road, Oxford, OX1 3SZ, United Kingdom. E-mail: klblake25@gmail.com

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Changing public behaviour is an essential step for successful conservation, and can be achieved through effective use of message framing. However, its use in the conservation sector is not well-studied. We first performed a content analysis to assess what types of framing styles environmental non-governmental organisations (ENGOs) often employ for their social media posts. We then ran a real-world online fundraising campaign to examine the influence of value-framing (‘Intrinsic’ and ‘Extrinsic’) and message valence (‘Positive’ and ‘Negative’) on audience engagement with the advertisements, across five countries. Altogether, ENGOs generally used ‘Positive’ framing for their posts significantly more often than ‘Negative’, but did not use one type of value-framing more than the other. For the fundraising campaign, there were significant differences between countries’ engagement with the advertisements. However, click-through rates did not significantly differ when using types of value-framing nor message valence, and no donations were received to support the campaign. These results may show that message valence and value-framing alone have little influence on audience engagement, if any, at least in the context of social media. To enhance campaign success for the future, it is recommended that conservationists offer concrete information regarding fundraising outcomes, and activate social norms.

Keywords: Western Ghats, conservation marketing, biodiversity, multinational comparison, message intervention

Non-Technical Summary

Background

Environmental problems are largely caused by human actions; therefore, changing public behaviour is an essential step for successful conservation. To motivate public engagement with this issue, conservationists may use ‘framing’ (a communication device used to emphasise specific aspects of a message). Two approaches to this in conservation communications include using types of value-framing (where the extrinsic or intrinsic value of biodiversity is emphasised) and message valence (where the positive or negative outcomes of engaging with a subject are highlighted).

Why was this study done?

To date, the impact of using different types of value-framing and message valence to motivate pro-environmental behaviours is unclear. This could be due to a scarcity of experiments on this subject in the conservation sector, as well as methodological shortcomings of the existing research. As examples, studies would benefit from providing findings with increased external validity, including multinational comparison to avoid Western biases, and empirically gauging actual behavioural changes (rather than only attitudes and intentions). We therefore set out to address these limitations, to both provide more robust evidence on the use of framing for conservation communications, and help settle the debate on this matter.

What did the researchers do and find?

We first assessed what framing styles environmental non-governmental organisations (ENGOs) often employ for their social media posts. We then conducted a real-world online fundraising campaign (run across five countries, and focusing on the Western Ghats) on social media to examine how types of value-framing and message valence would influence user engagement with advertisements. Altogether, ENGOs used positive framing for their social media posts substantially more often than negative, but generally did not use one type of value-framing more than the other. For the fundraising campaign, there were significant differences between countries’ engagement with the advertisements: notably, users in India were most likely to click on the advertisements, whilst users in Western countries (United Kingdom and United States of America) were least likely. However, users did not show any substantial preferences for types of value-framing nor message valence when clicking on advertisements. Furthermore, the campaign did not yield any donations.

What do these findings mean?

This is one of the more methodologically robust studies to assess the impacts of message framing for biodiversity conservation. The large disparities in engagement rates between countries is an important finding, highlighting the need to account for differences between audiences when planning campaigns. Regarding the overall impact of framing, these results may show that message valence and value-framing alone have little influence on audience engagement, if any, at least in the context of social media. Finally, we discuss how other features of message design may promote the success of future online campaigns, such as providing concrete information regarding fundraising outcomes, as well as activating social norms.

Highlights

  • ENGOs across countries prefer using types of message valence, but not value-framing.

  • Engagement with conservation advertisements significantly differs across countries.

  • Framing does not impact click-through rates, nor elicit donations, on social media.

  • Engagement rates do not support ENGOs’ preferred use of framing styles.

  • The impact of message framing for conservation campaigns may be overstated.

Message Framing for Environmental Topics

Environmental problems are largely caused by humans, so changing public behaviour is essential for successful conservation (Reddy et al., 2017; Schultz, 2011). To attract public attention to environmental issues and motivate positive engagement, conservationists may employ ‘message framing’ – a communication device used to emphasise specific aspects of a message, and change how it is perceived (Kolandai-Matchett & Armoudian, 2020; Li & Su, 2018).

One common framing style that has received much attention is use of message valence (see Bolsen, 2011; Bortree et al., 2012; Özgen et al., 2015; Walker et al., 2019), where the positive or negative outcomes of engaging with a subject are highlighted (Li et al., 2021; Maheswaran & Meyers-Levy, 1990). Additionally, communications may include value-framing (see Blackmore et al., 2013; Maze et al., 2016; Pascual et al., 2022; Uggla, 2018) to outline the extrinsic or intrinsic value of nature and its protection. As examples, extrinsic framing may prioritise financial incentives, ecosystem services, or social image, whereas intrinsic framing could emphasise an appreciation for nature being beautiful or having value for its own sake (Blackmore et al., 2013; Pascual et al., 2022). Movements such as Earth Optimism (https://www.earthoptimism.cambridgeconservation.org/) and Conservation Optimism (https://conservationoptimism.org/) have promoted the use of positive framing to emphasise the value of nature, and the importance of conservation work. However, it has been suggested that messages are often not constructed based on robust evidence (Kidd, Garrard, et al., 2019).

To date, the impact of using different frames remains unclear (Florence et al., 2022; Li & Su, 2018; Stadlthanner et al., 2022). Although some studies on environmental topics advocate the use of negative framing (see Gómez-Carmona et al., 2021; Li et al., 2021), others favour positive framing (see Chi et al., 2021; Spence & Pidgeon, 2010; Zubair et al., 2020). For biodiversity conservation specifically, some studies show that types of valence do not significantly differ in their influence on message recipients’ behaviour (e.g., donating to conservation charities, plastic bag usage, and interactions with coral reefs and wildlife whilst snorkelling; Nelson et al., 2020; Nelson et al., 2021). Yet, others suggest audiences indeed respond best to positive-framed messages (e.g., improving opinions and tolerance of wildlife, as well as willingness to donate to environmental organisations; Ballejo et al., 2021; Casola et al., 2020; Jacobson et al., 2019). This contrasts with theories of negativity bias in information processing (Taylor, 1991), whereby individuals tend to exhibit loss aversion: when facing equivalent positive and negative messages, the impact of losses is more persuasive (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), especially when recipients aim to minimise monetary or psychological losses (Tsai, 2007). Rather, it is arguable that people only have a limited capacity for concern (Nordhaus & Shellenberger, 2009), so they may deliberately avoid negative information, and thus psychological discomfort (i.e., the “ostrich effect” in economics; Karlsson et al., 2009). This could be particularly true for individuals who have low involvement with the message topic, and have less reason to fully evaluate the outcomes of how they respond (Meyers-Levy & Maheswaran, 2004). Therefore, conservation messages that emphasise loss may overall inhibit pro-environmental action (Jacobson et al., 2019).

A lack of consensus on the most effective employment of framing styles has also emerged for the use value-framing. Some researchers have found extrinsic frames effective to improve participants’ pro-environmental attitudes, intentions, and behaviour (Ropret Homar & Cvelbar, 2021), such as for purchasing green products (Segev et al., 2015). Others have found no differences between the effectiveness of the frames for motivating target behaviours (Steinhorst et al., 2015), or recommended using a combination of both framing styles (Mueller & Maes, 2015). However, intrinsic framing has been favoured to motivate pro-environmental behaviour (Bayram, 2012; Blackmore et al., 2013; Pelletier & Sharp, 2008), and advocated for use to reflect traditional conservation principles (Fisher & Brown, 2014). The overjustification hypothesis (based on self-perception theory; Bem, 1967), may support this preference. If message recipients attribute their behaviour to the potential for extrinsic rewards (e.g., financial incentives), rather than intrinsic motivations, they may show less willingness to behave pro-socially in order to avoid perceiving themselves as greedy (Bénabou & Tirole, 2006; Bolderdijk et al., 2013). Therefore, even where both extrinsic and intrinsic benefits are legitimate motivations to improve pro-environmental behaviour, it may be more effective to focus on intrinsic framing, and leave obvious financial implications unsaid (Schwartz et al., 2015). In addition to the argument of self-perception theory, intrinsic framing may be more effective to use because pro-environmental behaviours often return to a baseline level when extrinsic motivators are no longer available (Lehman & Geller, 2004; van der Linden, 2015). In contrast, intrinsic framing arguably leads to deeper engagement with, and processing of, message information (Vansteenkiste et al., 2006; Pelletier & Sharp, 2008), increased persistence with learning about a subject (such as recycling and ecology; Vansteenkiste et al., 2004), and behaviours linked to a genuine desire to protect the environment (Bayram, 2012).

Framing as a Knowledge Gap

Although practitioners may advocate positive and intrinsic framing, evidence regarding audience engagement with conservationists’ communication strategies is scarce (Kidd et al., 2019b; Kusmanoff, 2017), and methodological approaches in the past literature may underpin debate on these framing styles. Firstly, research on this topic has typically not reached the level of rigour demonstrated in other fields where message framing is commonly implemented (such as public health; Kidd, Garrard, et al., 2019), and suffers from low statistical power and external validity (Reddy et al., 2020). Secondly, the field would benefit from more multinational comparisons (Badullovich et al., 2020), since attitudes and behaviours are context-contingent and can vary greatly between Western and non-Western cultures (Riemer et al., 2014). Finally, much of the current research evaluating the influence of message framing for environmental topics fails to empirically gauge behavioural changes (Badullovich et al., 2020; Ropret Homar & Cvelbar, 2021; Nelson et al., 2020). This is problematic, given that participants’ attitudes and intentions are not necessarily indicative of their actual behaviours (Kollmuss & Agyeman, 2002; Seip & Strand, 1992). To address these limitations, it could be useful to advance recent work on how social media content may influence engagement (see Ballejo et al., 2021; Casola et al., 2020; Jacobson et al., 2019; Kubo et al., 2023; Shreedhar, 2021; Vu et al., 2019). Whilst previous research in this area has communicated messages using offline materials (e.g., posters and leaflets; Grazzini et al., 2018; Nelson et al., 2021; Salazar et al., 2022), audiences are increasingly sourcing information digitally (Jacobson et al., 2019). Therefore, using social media can efficiently expand the global reach of messages (Casola et al., 2020), easily supplying large sample sizes, allowing multinational comparisons, and improving external validity by observing recipients in a natural setting. Additionally, social media platforms facilitate measurements of behavioural changes, by tracking click-through rates and donations elicited by post content (Kubo et al., 2023; Shreedhar, 2021).

Altogether, it is clear that conservationists must generate more robust evidence on this matter, to better gauge the effectiveness of framing styles and therefore optimise their message designs (Kidd et al., 2019a; MacFarlane et al., 2022). The following research aims to accomplish this, to help settle the debate on the use of message framing in conservation communications. Two studies will be conducted. First, a content analysis to assess what framing styles environmental non-governmental organisations (ENGOs) are commonly employing for their social media posts. Second, an online field experiment to test which message frames (‘Negative’/‘Positive’ and ‘Extrinsic’/‘Intrinsic’) most effectively engage audiences with real-world ENGO appeals, measured through click-through rates (CTRs) and donations received. Based on the literature discussed, the following hypotheses are made:

  • H1: Advertisements will generally elicit higher CTRs when using ‘Intrinsic’ rather than ‘Extrinsic’ framing.

  • H2: Advertisements will generally elicit higher CTRs when using ‘Positive’ rather than ‘Negative’ framing.

  • H3: Advertisements will generally receive more donations when using ‘Intrinsic’ rather than ‘Extrinsic’ framing.

  • H4: Advertisements will generally receive more donations when using ‘Positive’ rather than ‘Negative’ framing.

Study 1: Content Analysis

Method

To understand how ENGOs frame their messages on social media, we performed a content analysis on their Facebook posts. Studied organisations were members of the International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN); as of February 2022, 102 international ENGOs and 1,046 national ENGOs were Members. To allow multinational comparison, studied ENGOs were located across the following five countries: United Kingdom (UK), United States of America (USA), South Africa, India, and Brazil. The top five organisations (by number of Facebook followers) in each country were analysed, leading to a total of 25 ENGOs included in the content analysis (see Table A1 Appendix 1). Data collection was conducted between November 21, 2021 and January 28, 2022. For each organisation, the content included for analysis consisted of a maximum of 100 posts, uploaded within the previous 12 months, that predominantly used: either ‘Intrinsic’ or ‘Extrinsic’ value-framing and either ‘Positive’ or ‘Negative’ valence. In this context, the message frame styles were defined as:

  • ‘Intrinsic’ (an entity is inherently valuable, regardless of any use it may have to others).

  • ‘Extrinsic’ (an entity is valued, and therefore worth protecting, because it is valuable to others).

  • ‘Positive’ (communicates the positive outcomes that could occur if audiences take action).

  • ‘Negative’ (warns audiences about the potential negative implications of not taking action).

Posts were assessed by a single coder. Examples where the use of framing was ambiguous were discussed with a second coder. We performed Chi-Square Goodness of Fit Tests to determine whether organisations’ social media posts generally favoured ‘Intrinsic’ or ‘Extrinsic’ frames, as well as ‘Positive’ or ‘Negative’ valence. Tests were performed using R (v4.2.2; R Core Team, 2022) in RStudio (Posit Team, 2022), with packages rcompanion (v2.4.18; Mangiafico, 2022) and DescTools (v0.99.47; Andri et al. 2022).

Results

The usage of message frame styles by country is shown in Table A2 (Appendix 2). The sample included 2,074 Facebook posts, with content from: the UK (20.11%), USA (16.20%), South Africa (24.11%), India (18.61%), and Brazil (20.97%).

Figure 1 shows how often framing styles were used within countries. Besides South Africa, countries more frequently focused on ‘Extrinsic’ than ‘Intrinsic’ framing for their posts. Only Brazil showed a significant difference in their use of value-framing (see Table 1 for statistics), but the effect size was small (Cohen, 1988). Compared to other countries, Brazil used ‘Extrinsic’ framing the most often (and ‘Intrinsic’ the least). ‘Positive’ frames were used significantly more often than ‘Negative’ by the UK, USA, South Africa, India, and Brazil, with all countries showing large effect sizes (Cohen, 1988). ‘Positive’ framing was used most often in the UK, and least often in the USA (where ‘Negative’ framing was used the most often compared to the other four countries). Overall, ‘Extrinsic Positive’ was the most employed framing style combination, followed by ‘Intrinsic Positive’, ‘Intrinsic Negative’, then ‘Extrinsic Negative’ (see Figure A1 Appendix 3).

Click to enlarge
gep.11181-f1
Figure 1

Usage of the Four Message Frame Styles Across the Five Countries

Note. The four message frame styles are Extrinsic, Intrinsic, Negative, Positive. Usage is measured as a percentage.

Table 1

Chi-Square Goodness of Fit Test Results for the Usage of Message Frame Styles Across All Five Countries

Country N X2 df 95% CIa
Cramer’s V
Extrinsic/Negative Intrinsic/Positive
Value-framing
Brazil 435 13.63*** 1 [54.25, 63.78] [36.55, 46.08] .18
India 386 1.04 1 [47.67, 57.96] [42.49, 52.78] .05
South Africa 500 3.20 1 [41.60, 50.65] [49.60, 58.65] .08
UK 417 1.06 1 [47.72, 57.63] [42.69, 52.60] .05
USA 336 3.44 1 [49.70, 60.66] [39.58, 50.54] .10
Message valence
Brazil 435 330.21*** 1 [4.37, 8.64] [91.49, 95.77] .87
India 386 191.67*** 1 [11.40, 18.21] [81.87, 88.68] .70
South Africa 500 320.00*** 1 [7.60, 12.59] [87.60, 92.59] .80
UK 417 330.07*** 1 [3.60, 7.63] [92.57, 96.60] .89
USA 336 92.19*** 1 [19.35, 28.36] [71.73, 80.74] .52

a95% CIs for extrinsic versus intrinsic value-framing, and negative versus positive message valence.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Study 2: Online Field Experiment

Method

To test how message framing can influence audience engagement and donation behaviour, we designed four advertisements for a British ENGO’s campaign to help protect the Western Ghats (one of the planet’s biodiversity hotspots; Sreekumar et al., 2020). Aside from exceptional natural beauty, the Western Ghats is home to many threatened species endemic to this region, including the Lion-Tailed Macaque (Macaca silenus), as well as some of the largest remaining populations megafauna, such as the Asian Elephant (Elephas maximus) and Tiger (Panthera tigris). The Western Ghats is also immensely valuable for sustaining the livelihoods of over 200 million people, by providing essential natural resources for medicine and sustenance (Sreekumar et al., 2020), alongside ecosystem services that maintain water security in the peninsular Indian states (Ramachandra & Bharath, 2020). Despite the abundance of reasons to preserve this landscape, only 10% of the Western Ghats is legally protected (Ramachandra & Bharath, 2020). Consequently, ongoing threats to this region (e.g., deforestation) pose social, economic, and ecological implications for all species who depend on it (Jha et al., 2000). Altogether, the four advertisements (see Figure 2 and Blake et al., 2023c) used this information to combine either ‘Extrinsic’ or ‘Intrinsic’ value-framing with ‘Negative’ or ‘Positive’ message valence. The presentation and organisation of text and visuals was inspired by materials used in previous research on framing environmental topics, such as plastic usage and protecting marine life (see Grazzini et al., 2018; Nelson et al., 2020; Nelson et al., 2021). All advertisements were 12 seconds in length, and used identical visuals and composition (with only the four types of framing styles differentiating them). Additionally, advertisements were available with both 4:5 and 9:16 aspect ratios to ensure the messages could be correctly displayed across devices and social media platforms. A ‘Donate Now’ button was embedded into each advertisement, made using a JustGiving.com plugin. When clicking this button, a pop-up window opened to provide options for users to financially support the campaign.

Click to enlarge
gep.11181-f2
Figure 2

Screenshots of the Advertisements Using Types of Message Valence (X-Axis) And Value-Framing (Y-Axis)

The research design was a 2 x 2 between-subjects experiment, using voluntary sampling. To strive to create more comparable sample sizes across conditions, we set up all four campaigns on a web—“traffic”—objective (where advertisements are shown to users most likely to click on them), based on the auction buying type with the same budget. The advertisement delivery was optimised for link clicks with no cost-per-result goal. In order to test a sample as representative as possible of potential donors, the target audience for all campaigns was selected to be adults (at least 18 years old) of all demographics, interests, and behaviours. Users’ personal information was not collected, so a more extensive sample description is not available. Advertisements were presented in English when shown in the USA, UK, India, and South Africa, and in Portuguese when shown in Brazil. Advertisements were run from March 21 to 30, 2022; placements where advertisements were displayed were selected manually in the standard inventory, and excluded those available outside of Facebook and Instagram (as well as Facebook Right Column, Instagram Shop, Messenger Inbox, and Facebook Group Feed). To ensure that the adverts were displayed in equivalent environments, they were presented on: Facebook’s Feed, Marketplace, Stories, Instant Articles, Video Feed, and Messenger Stories, as well as Instagram’s Feed, Explore, Stories, and Reels. Furthermore, advertisements could be seen on both social media platforms’ in-stream (as skippable ads in videos), or found using the Search bars. The total budget allocated to this campaign was divided between the four advertisements; these were removed from both Facebook and Instagram once all money was spent, and data collection ended.

The dataset was obtained using Meta Ads Manager. For each of the four advertisements, the key outcome variables collected for analysis were click-through rates (CTRs) and donations (the total financial value of donations received from users). To calculate CTRs, clicks to website (the number of times users followed a hyperlink to go to the donation webpage) were divided by impressions (the number of times adverts were presented on-screen to users). This engagement behaviour was measured because link clicks on advertisements could be interpreted as donation intentions (Jilke et al., 2019). To calculate the overall CTR for advertisements using ‘Extrinsic’ framing, we combined the results for ‘Extrinsic Negative’ and ‘Extrinsic Positive’. To calculate the overall CTR for advertisements using ‘Negative’ framing, we combined the results for ‘Extrinsic Negative’ and ‘Intrinsic Negative’. The same processes were used for ‘Intrinsic’ and ‘Positive’ framing. Other variables regarding users’ social actions were also measured (see Table A3 Appendix 4).

We used beta regression to test the hypotheses and predict the effect of framing across countries. Dummy variables were created for message valence, value-framing, and country; the reference levels used for comparison were ‘Positive’ framing, ‘Intrinsic’ framing, and the USA, respectively. Beta regression was performed using R (v3.6.0 Mangiafico, 2022) in R Studio (Posit Team, 2022).

Results

The number of impressions and clicks to website for the frames across each country can be seen in Table A4 (see Appendix 5). The predicted effects of framing styles on CTRs across countries, according to the regression analysis, can be seen in Figure 3, Table 2, and Table 3. A significant main effect of country on CTRs was found: CTRs were significantly highest in India, and lowest in the UK (followed by the USA). Results for Brazil and South Africa did not significantly differ from each other. The influence of frames on CTRs within countries varied, but there were not any significant differences in the effectiveness of message frames (except in India, where ‘Intrinsic Positive’ messages yielded significantly higher CTRs than ‘Extrinsic Negative’). Although CTRs were generally higher when advertisements used ‘Intrinsic’ and ‘Positive’ framing, there were not significant main effects of value-framing nor message valence on CTRs. Therefore, hypotheses H1 and H2 (that advertisements would elicit higher CTRs when using ‘Intrinsic’ rather than ‘Extrinsic’, and ‘Positive’ rather than ‘Negative’, framing) were not supported.

Regardless of the differences in CTRs for each of the adverts across all five countries, no donations were received in response to any of the advertisements to support the Western Ghats campaign. As such, neither hypothesis H3 nor H4 (regarding a greater effect being found for ‘Intrinsic’ value-framing and ‘Positive’ message valence to attract donations) was supported.

Click to enlarge
gep.11181-f3
Figure 3

A Point Plot Showing the Predicted Effect of Four Types of Message Frames on CTRs (With Error Bars), Across the Five Countries

Table 2

Regression Results Showing the Predicted Click-Through Rates for Adverts Shown Across the Five Countries

Country Message Frame Style
Extrinsic Negative
Extrinsic Positive
Intrinsic Negative
Intrinsic Positive
Pred. 95% CI Pred. 95% CI Pred. 95% CI Pred. 95% CI
Brazil 0.02 [.02, .03] 0.02 [.02, .03] 0.02 [.02, .03] 0.02 [.02, .03]
India 0.03 [.03, .04] 0.04 [.04, .05] 0.04 [.03, .05] 0.05 [.04, .06]
South Africa 0.02 [.02, .03] 0.02 [.02, .03] 0.02 [.02, .03] 0.02 [.02, .03]
UK 0.01 [.01, .01] 0.01 [.01, .01] 0.01 [.01, .01] 0.01 [.01, .01]
USA 0.01 [.01, .02] 0.01 [.01, .02] 0.01 [.01, .02] 0.01 [.01, .02]

Note. Pred. = Predicted.

Table 3

Regression Results for the Effect of Message Framing and Country on Click-Through Rates

Variable Coefficient
Negative -0.05 (0.11)
Extrinsic -0.02 (0.11)
Brazil 0.35** (0.12)
India 1.21*** (0.12)
South Africa 0.39*** (0.12)
UK -0.57*** (0.15)
Negative x Brazil 0.09 (0.14)
Negative x India -0.16 (0.13)
Negative x South Africa 0.12 (0.14)
Negative x UK 0.02 (0.18)
Brazil x Extrinsic 0.08 (0.14)
India x Extrinsic -0.12 (0.12)
South Africa x Extrinsic 0.07 (0.14)
UK x Extrinsic 0.05 (0.18)

Note. Standard errors reported in parentheses.

**p < .01. ***p < .001.

Discussion

This research explored the use of framing in conservation advertisements across countries, and contributes to the ongoing debate about the impact of framing styles to influence public support for biodiversity conservation. Overall, the content analysis revealed no significant preferences in ENGOs’ use of value-framing (besides in Brazil), but ‘Positive’ message valence was favoured across all countries. Regarding the online campaign, although advertisements using ‘Intrinsic’ and ‘Positive’ framing received higher CTRs than those using ‘Extrinsic’ and ‘Negative’ framing, these differences were not statistically significant. Furthermore, no donations were received.

The Use of Frames for Conservation Messages

Differences Across Countries

In contrast to past literature on conservationists’ use of framing (e.g., Blackmore et al., 2013; Bolsen, 2011; Bortree et al., 2012; Pascual et al., 2022), our content analysis revealed that ENGOs across all countries overwhelmingly favoured use of ‘Positive’ message valence, but typically did not strongly prefer either ‘Intrinsic’ or ‘Extrinsic’ value-framing. This finding may partially support claims that conservation messages are often not constructed based on robust evidence (Kidd, Garrard, et al., 2019), as differences in CTRs were not statistically significant, and no donations were received for any of the advertisements. Thus, perhaps these framing styles make little difference on audience engagement, and do not support the disproportionate use of ‘Positive’ over ‘Negative’ framing by all countries.

Although the use of framing styles across countries was generally similar, it can also be helpful to understand how the effectiveness of framing differs for audiences across geographical and cultural contexts (Vu et al., 2019). This is strategic when planning future campaigns, given their substantial cost implications. Because many experimental studies on environmental framing have focused primarily on Western countries (Badullovich et al., 2020; Florence et al., 2022), the inclusion of multinational comparison was a strength of our study, and the following could explain the results. Firstly, Western cultures (notably, the USA and UK) have often shown self-interest and independence (Kohls, 1984; Schultz & Zelezny, 2003; Triandis, 1996), whereas Eastern cultures instead promote interdependence and relatedness to others (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). If such values persist today, perhaps this partly underpins why the UK and USA had comparatively lower CTRs than other countries. Adding to this, perceptions of conservation actions could be linked to the extent that they impact the lives of local residents (Sandbrook et al., 2019), and engagement with messages on climate change has been higher when focusing on local rather than global consequences (Scannell & Gifford, 2013). Taken together, perhaps this explains why lower-income countries (i.e., Brazil, South Africa, and India) showed significantly higher CTRs than higher-income countries (i.e., UK and USA). Furthermore, users’ closer proximity to the Western Ghats may explain why CTRs were highest in India.

Differences Between Frames

The results for CTRs are consistent with past literature on this subject, where engagement with conservation messages was not significantly influenced by using different types of message valence (see Nelson et al., 2020; Nelson et al., 2021), nor value-framing (see Steinhorst et al., 2015). On the one hand, these null results support recent suggestions that the impact of framing is extremely small, if existent (Szaszi et al., 2022). Perhaps, as Li and Su (2018) considered, the influence of message framing on outcome variables has been overstated due to publication bias—where social science experiments with statistically significant results can be as much as three times more likely to be published in peer-reviewed journals than those with null results (Franco et al., 2014). Alternatively, it should be acknowledged that the number of repeated exposures needed for advertisements to be effective can vary (Schmidt & Eisend, 2015), so under-scaled studies may explain non-significant influences on behaviours (Doughty et al., 2021). Since the results did move in the expected direction (where ‘Intrinsic’ and ‘Positive’ framing led to higher CTRs than the alternative framing styles), perhaps repeating this experiment for a longer time period could provide a larger sample size and enhance the frequency of exposures recipients have to messages, to potentially uncover small but statistically significant effects. To further reflect on our research design, even though field experiments can enhance the external validity of findings (Borah & Xiao, 2018), future work could benefit from being complemented by laboratory studies (as seen in Grazzini et al., 2018). Thus far, there appears to be almost twice as many observational than experimental studies on framing environmental topics (see Badullovich et al., 2020). Yet, controlled settings can allow researchers to better understand the mechanisms that drive effects (Gneezy, 2017), such as differences in audience demographics and cognition. Specific to our research, follow-up studies would benefit from including a control group which is exposed to an advertisement that does not use message framing. As seen in the experiments by Nelson et al. (2020) and Nelson et al. (2021), this would offer insight into whether using any framing styles (even if they do not significantly differ from each other) is clearly more effective at influencing target behaviours than not using framing styles at all.

It is also important to explore why no donations were received (given that link clicks on advertisements can signify donation intentions; Jilke et al., 2019). This result has similarities with other recent studies: Shreedhar (2021) found that conservation campaigns on social media may not attract donations regardless of differences in CTRs, whilst a similar study by Kubo et al. (2023) received a very low amount in donations relative to their number of impressions and donation page views. CTRs have therefore been judged as inappropriate proxies for behaviours such as donations (Kubo et al., 2023; Shreedhar, 2021); because intentions are not necessarily indicative of actual behaviours (Kollmuss & Agyeman, 2002; Seip & Strand, 1992), relying solely on such metrics can result in misleading overestimates of message impacts (Doughty et al., 2021). Hence, measuring donations was a strength of this research: a multi-pronged evaluation of impacts allows for more accurate interpretation of whether messages were successful (Doughty et al., 2021). From such an approach it can clearly be deduced that regardless of how types of message valence or value-framing may differ in their appeal, more needs to be done to strategically enhance the success of conservation messages across countries, and transform CTRs into actual donations. The use of multinational comparison also reinforces this conclusion: despite the main effect of country for message engagement (where India yielded significantly higher rates than other countries), proximity to the Western Ghats also did not motivate monetary support.

How Conservation Messaging Could Be Improved

Our investigation addresses frequently stated limitations of past studies (particularly, by including measurements of actual behaviours rather than attitudes or intentions). Notably, it benefitted from evaluating the impact of a real online campaign across multiple countries; the use of online advertising is an increasingly important means for fundraising, and the potential revenue from this is set to rise (Lacetera et al., 2016). However, given that successful fundraising is essential for conservation efforts, it must be explored why the advertisements in this experiment did not attract monetary support. It is plausible that this occurred because the advertisements primarily focused on raising audiences’ awareness about the value of the Western Ghats—consistent with a traditional and commonly employed communication strategy, based on the ‘knowledge-deficit model’ (Kidd, Garrard, et al., 2019). This approach is predicated on the assumption that message recipients will change their behaviour if given the right information (Schultz, 2002). Whilst it is reasonable to expect that raising awareness about biodiversity is an important part of conservation messaging (Kidd, Garrard, et al., 2019), the evidence presented here supports arguments that conservationists should not solely depend on this model (see Schultz, 2002; Schultz, 2011; Toomey et al., 2017). Therefore, in addition to raising public awareness about biodiversity using types of value-framing and message valence, these findings demonstrate that conservationists must consider what else these messages need to be effective.

One approach to improve audiences’ responses to conservation messages could be using more concrete, rather than abstract, information. Simply put, this refers to how someone may take action, rather than why they should take action, respectively (Grazzini et al., 2018; Zhu et al., 2017). Concrete messages that transparently provide detailed information on how donations will be implemented are perceived as more credible, and are associated with increased message and organisational trustworthiness (Xiao et al., 2022). Given that source trustworthiness is a critical antecedent to donation intentions (Wiencierz et al., 2015), fundraising initiatives should provide potential donors with concrete information. Past research has shown that messages with more detailed fundraising outcomes (i.e., specific details on how donations could help to provide support for families fighting cancer) led to greater intentions to donate than those with more abstract information (Xiao et al., 2022), and that the influence of message valence becomes more pronounced when using concrete information (Grazzini et al., 2018). The advertisements designed for the Western Ghats campaign arguably focused considerably more on abstract information, and the donation page on JustGiving.com did not offer any details on how donations would be used. Combined, perhaps the lack of detailed information regarding fundraising outcomes contributed to why no donations were received. For future research, conservationists should consider the role that such approaches may have for the effectiveness of message designs, especially when striving to accomplish more than raising awareness on a subject.

A second explanation for the lack of donations stems from the use of social media. The effectiveness of messaging could be leveraged using social influence (White et al., 2019), as people may engage in pro-social behaviours due to concerns regarding their social image (Kristofferson et al., 2014). However, social media offers costless opportunities to show support for campaigns: exhibiting an “illusion of activism”, users can support a cause by “liking” or “favouring” Facebook or Twitter posts, without needing to donate or engage further (Kristofferson et al., 2014; Lacetera et al., 2016). Indeed, this issue was evidenced in a large-scale study by Lacetera et al. (2016), as well as the current research (where advertisements did not attract donations, but received a substantial number of social actions such as “likes”, especially in India). Nevertheless, effective use of social influence could prove an asset for enhancing message success on social media. As examples, participants have generated more charitable donations (via clicks on a keyboard) when required to publicly announce how many times they clicked (Ariely et al., 2009), and have been more likely to support a cause when knowing others’ contributions (Kubo et al., 2018). Moreover, ‘seed money’ frames, where messages announce what percentage of a fundraising target has already been reached, have been particularly effective at attracting donations for real-world campaigns (Kubo et al., 2018; Kubo et al., 2023). It has been suggested that progress bars for fundraising and deadlines can allow donors to gauge the success of campaigns and motivate donations (Choy & Schlagwein, 2016), and their use has therefore been recommended for fundraising on social media (Yoganathan et al., 2021). This research did not aim to test impacts of social influence, but it would be advantageous for future investigations to focus on this. For instance, conveying descriptive social norms (telling people what others do) may provoke conformity (Kubo et al., 2023; List & Lucking-Reiley, 2002), whilst expressing injunctive social norms (indicating what is commonly approved or disapproved of) could activate a sense of social responsibility to motivate target conservation behaviours (Thompson et al. 2015).

Using ‘Positive’ and ‘Intrinsic’ Framing

Given our findings, it could be beneficial for practitioners and future research to prioritise other framing styles and contextual information when communicating about biodiversity conservation. As examples, appealing to emotions, morality, public health, societal versus personal outcomes, or spatial and temporal distance from environmental issues has been popular in the literature on message framing (Kolandai-Matchett & Armoudian, 2020; Li & Su, 2018; Ropret Homar & Cvelbar, 2021). Nevertheless, exploring how to successfully employ ‘Intrinsic’ versus ‘Extrinsic’ value-framing and ‘Positive’ versus ‘Negative’ message valence is arguably still worthwhile. Messages about conservation that emphasise loss can inhibit pro-environmental action (Jacobson et al., 2019), perhaps because recipients may deliberately avoid negative information that would otherwise lead to psychological discomfort (Karlsson et al., 2009). In contrast, optimistic messages can be unifying and empowering—inspiring hope and collective action to achieve pro-environmental outcomes (de Lange et al., 2022; McAfee et al., 2019). Such an idea is fundamental to Common Cause (Blackmore et al., 2013), an organisation who advised that ENGOs should strive to promote collective activism and avoid disempowering messages that reduce recipients to merely passive sources of income. However, it should be noted that optimistic appeals can risk complacency, where message recipients may deduce that there is less need for personal action (Hornsey & Fielding, 2016; Salazar et al., 2022). Conservationists have been advised to be clear that individual and collective support for conservation efforts remains vital (Brosch, 2021), but further research on this matter is needed to better understand the effectiveness of such an approach.

Regarding value-framing, conservationists have been criticised for prioritising the value of ecosystem services and economic growth to attract support for funding and policymaking (Fisher & Brown, 2014; Redford & Adams, 2009). Specifically, appealing to financial motivations could be problematic because this may transform moral or social issues into an economic trade-off (rather than being about the greater good; Handgraaf et al., 2013). Conservationists have therefore been encouraged to instead base their messages on the intrinsic value of the natural world (Blackmore et al., 2013), to elicit behaviours linked to a genuine desire to protect the environment (Bayram, 2012). However, the cultural heterogeneity in how audiences respond to environmental messages must be recognised (Sandbrook et al., 2019), where there can be varying tendencies to prefer self-transcendent (biospheric or altruistic), or self-enhancing (egoistic) value orientations (see De Groot & Steg, 2007; Schultz & Zelezny, 2003). Our experiment did not address this, so future research could assess this effect using framing styles detailed by Common Cause. As examples, messages may inspire action based on peoples’ connection to nature and each other, or an appreciation of nature’s beauty and potential for discovery (Blackmore et al., 2013). Although Common Cause argued that messages should avoid using extrinsic motivators related to organisations’ and individuals’ power or image, we again propose that it could be beneficial to explore whether activating social norms further affects message engagement. For example, farmers have been more willing to show pro-environmental behaviours, without monetary payment, when driven by social influence (Vaske et al., 2020). Unlike financial incentives, using social influence is advantageous here because social rewards do not require environmental groups to be better funded, nor do they crowd out intrinsic motivations (Handgraaf et al., 2013). Therefore, this provides an opportunity for conservationists to spread awareness of the inherent value of biodiversity, whilst also appealing to those who may be more influenced by extrinsic motivators like self-enhancement.

Conclusion

Framing is often employed to influence engagement with environmental messages, but there is ongoing debate regarding its effectiveness. Our investigation contributes novel evidence on this matter, whilst simultaneously addressing a variety of limitations presented in past studies. Overall, our results showed that ENGOs generally favoured ‘Positive’ framing for their posts over ‘Negative’, but did not use one type of value-framing significantly more often than the other. However, despite significant differences in how countries engaged with the advertisements, the results did not reveal significant differences in the effectiveness of types of value-framing nor message valence for audience engagement with the online fundraising campaign. This research offers strategies for practitioners to consider when using these framing styles in the future. Conservationists are encouraged to further explore this field: it is critical that messages are optimally designed to attract public support for efforts that address ongoing ecological devastation.

Openness and Transparency Statements

The present article has been checked by its handling editor(s) for compliance with the journal's open science and transparency policies. The completed Transparency Checklist is publicly available at: https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.13279

Author Contributions

Katie Blake: Methodology | Resources | Investigation | Formal analysis | Visualization | Writing – original draft.

Takahiro Kubo: Methodology | Writing – review & editing.

Diogo Veríssimo: Conceptualization | Methodology | Investigation | Formal analysis | Funding acquisition | Supervision | Writing – review & editing.

Acknowledgments

We had support of a Social Media practitioner during data collection.

Funding

The research was funded by a UK charity focused on nature conservation. The charity has requested to stay anonymous. The funder did not interfere with the research process.

Competing Interests

Diogo Veríssimo is on the Editorial Board of GEP.

Ethics Statement

This research involved observation of online public behaviour. It did not include interaction with individual participants nor collection of personal information. Thus, ethical approval was not required.

Diversity

In the list below, the check mark () indicates which steps were taken to increase diversity within the context of this paper. Steps that were not taken or did not apply are unmarked ().

Ethnically or otherwise diverse sample(s)

Gender balanced sample(s)

Inclusive gender measure

Inclusive materials

Sampling justification

Extensive sample description

Discussion of generalizability

Diverse reference list

Underprivileged / minority author(s)

Early career author(s)

Degree of privilege/marginalization considered in authorship order

Author(s) from sampled population (avoiding ’helicopter science’)

Supplementary Materials

The following table provides an overview of the accessibility of supplementary materials (if any) for this paper.

Type of supplementary materials Availability/Access
Data Blake et al., 2023a
Code Blake et al., 2023b
Material Blake et al., 2023c
Study/Analysis preregistration The reported studies were not preregistered.

Openess and Transparency Badges

Open data: YES.

Open code: YES.

Open materials: YES.

Preregistration: NO.

Diversity statement: YES.

Note: YES = the present article meets the criteria for awarding the badge. NO = the present article does not meet the criteria for awarding the badge or the criteria are not applicable.

References

  • Andri, S., Aho, K., Alfons, A., Anderegg, N., Aragon, T., Arachchige, C., Arppe, A., Baddeley, A., Barton, K., Bolker, B., Borchers, H. W., Caeiro, F., Champely, S., Chessel, D., Chhay, L., Cooper, N., Cummins, C., Dewey, M., Doran, H. C., … Zeileis, A. (2022). DescTools: Tools for descriptive statistics: R package version 0.99.47. R Project. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=DescTools

  • Ariely, D., Bracha, A., & Meier, S. (2009). Doing good or doing well? Image motivation and monetary incentives in behaving prosocially. American Economic Review, 99(1), 544-555. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.99.1.544

  • Badullovich, N., Grant, W. J., & Colvin, R. M. (2020). Framing climate change for effective communication: A systematic map. Environmental Research Letters, 15(12), Article 123002. https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/aba4c7

  • Ballejo, F., Plaza, P. I., & Lambertucci, S. A. (2021). Framing of visual content shown on popular social media may affect viewers’ attitudes to threatened species. Scientific Reports, 11(1), Article 13512. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-92815-7

  • Bayram, S. A. (2012). On the role of intrinsic value in terms of environmental education. Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences, 47, 1087-1091. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.06.783

  • Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74(3), 183-200. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024835

  • Bénabou, R., & Tirole, J. (2006). Incentives and prosocial behavior. American Economic Review, 96(5), 1652-1678. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.96.5.1652

  • Blackmore, E., Underhill, R., McQuilkin, J., & Leach, R. (2013). Common cause for nature: finding values and frames in the conservation sector. Public Interest Research Centre. https://publicinterest.org.uk/new-report-common-cause-for-nature/

  • Blake, K., Kubo, T., & Veríssimo, D. (2023a). Data for: Measuring the effectiveness of value-framing and message valence on audience engagement across countries [Data]. PsychOpen GOLD. https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.12925

  • Blake, K., Kubo, T., & Veríssimo, D. (2023b). Code for: Measuring the effectiveness of value-framing and message valence on audience engagement across countries [Code]. PsychOpen GOLD. https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.12924

  • Blake, K., Kubo, T., & Veríssimo, D. (2023c). Moving Image for: Measuring the effectiveness of value-framing and message valence on audience engagement across countries [Moving image]. PsychOpen GOLD. https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.12927

  • Bolderdijk, J. W., Steg, L., Geller, E. S., Lehman, P. K., & Postmes, T. (2013). Comparing the effectiveness of monetary versus moral motives in environmental campaigning. Nature Climate Change, 3(4), 413-416. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1767

  • Bolsen, T. (2011). The construction of news: Energy crises, advocacy messages, and frames toward conservation. International Journal of Press/Politics, 16(2), 143-162. https://doi.org/10.1177/1940161210392782

  • Borah, P., & Xiao, X. (2018). The importance of ‘likes’: The interplay of message framing, source, and social endorsement on credibility perceptions of health information on Facebook. Journal of Health Communication, 23(4), 399-411. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2018.1455770

  • Bortree, D., Ahern, L., Dou, X., & Smith, A. N. (2012). Framing environmental advocacy: A study of 30 years of advertising in National Geographic Magazine. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 17(2), 77-91. https://doi.org/10.1002/nvsm.437

  • Brosch, T. (2021). Affect and emotions as drivers of climate change perception and action: A review. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 42, 15-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.001

  • Casola, W. R., Rushing, J., Futch, S., Vayer, V., Lawson, D. F., Cavalieri, M. J., Larson, L. R., & Peterson, M. N. (2020). How do YouTube videos impact tolerance of wolves? Human Dimensions of Wildlife, 25(6), 531-543. https://doi.org/10.1080/10871209.2020.1773582

  • Chi, O. H., Denton, G., & Gursoy, D. (2021). Interactive effects of message framing and information content on carbon offsetting behaviors. Tourism Management, 83, Article 104244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2020.104244

  • Choy, K., & Schlagwein, D. (2016). Crowdsourcing for a better world: On the relation between IT affordances and donor motivations in charitable crowdfunding. Information Technology & People, 29(1), 221-247. https://doi.org/10.1108/ITP-09-2014-0215

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Routledge.https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587

  • De Groot, J. I., & Steg, L. (2007). Value orientations and environmental beliefs in five countries: Validity of an instrument to measure egoistic, altruistic and biospheric value orientations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(3), 318-332. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022107300278

  • de Lange, E., Sharkey, W., Castelló y Tickell, S., Migné, J., Underhill, R., & Milner-Gulland, E. J. (2022). Communicating the biodiversity crisis: From “warnings” to positive engagement. Tropical Conservation Science 15, . https://doi.org/10.1177/19400829221134893

  • Doughty, H., Milner-Gulland, E. J., Lee, J. S. H., Oliver, K., Carrasco, L. R., & Veríssimo, D. (2021). Evaluating a large-scale online behaviour change intervention aimed at wildlife product consumers in Singapore. PLoS One, 16(3), Article e0248144. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248144

  • Fisher, J. A., & Brown, K. (2014). Ecosystem services concepts and approaches in conservation: Just a rhetorical tool? Ecological Economics, 108, 257-265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2014.11.004

  • Florence, E. S., Fleischman, D., Mulcahy, R., & Wynder, M. (2022). Message framing effects on sustainable consumer behaviour: A systematic review and future research directions for social marketing. Journal of Social Marketing, 12(4), 623-652. https://doi.org/10.1108/JSOCM-09-2021-0221

  • Franco, A., Malhotra, N., & Simonovits, G. (2014). Publication bias in the social sciences: Unlocking the file drawer. Science, 345(6203), 1502-1505. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1255484

  • Gneezy, A. (2017). Field experimentation in marketing research. Journal of Marketing Research, 54(1), 140-143. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.16.0225

  • Gómez-Carmona, D., Muñoz-Leiva, F., Liébana-Cabanillas, F., Nieto-Ruiz, A., Martínez-Fiestas, M., & Campoy, C. (2021). The effect of consumer concern for the environment, self-regulatory focus and message framing on green advertising effectiveness: An eye tracking study. Environmental Communication, 15(6), 813-841. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2021.1914701

  • Grazzini, L., Rodrigo, P., Aiello, G., & Viglia, G. (2018). Loss or gain? The role of message framing in hotel guests’ recycling behaviour. Journal of Sustainable Tourism, 26(11), 1944-1966. https://doi.org/10.1080/09669582.2018.1526294

  • Handgraaf, M. J. J., Van Lidth de Jeude, M. A., & Appelt, K. C. (2013). Public praise vs. private pay: Effects of rewards on energy conservation in the workplace. Ecological Economics, 86, 86-92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2012.11.008

  • Hornsey, M. J., & Fielding, K. S. (2016). A cautionary note about messages of hope: Focusing on progress in reducing carbon emissions weakens mitigation motivation. Global Environmental Change, 39, 26-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2016.04.003

  • Jacobson, S. K., Morales, N. A., Chen, B., Soodeen, R., Moulton, M. P., & Jain, E. (2019). Love or loss: Effective message framing to promote environmental conservation. Applied Environmental Education and Communication, 18(3), 252-265. https://doi.org/10.1080/1533015X.2018.1456380

  • Jha, C. S., Dutt, C. B. S., & Bawa, K. S. (2000). Deforestation and land use changes in Western Ghats, India. Current Science, 79(2), 231-238. https://www.jstor.org/stable/24103455

  • Jilke, S., Lu, J., Xu, C., & Shinohara, S. (2019). Using large-scale social media experiments in public administration: Assessing charitable consequences of government funding of nonprofits. Journal of Public Administration: Research and Theory, 29(4), 627-639. https://doi.org/10.1093/jopart/muy021

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185

  • Karlsson, N., Loewenstein, G., & Seppi, D. (2009). The ostrich effect: Selective attention to information. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 38(2), 95-115. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-009-9060-6

  • Kidd, L. R., Bekessy, S. A., & Garrard, G. E. (2019a). Evidence is key for effective biodiversity communication. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 34(8), 693-694. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2019.05.010

  • Kidd, L. R., Bekessy, S. A., & Garrard, G. E. (2019b). Neither hope nor fear: Empirical evidence should drive biodiversity conservation strategies. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 34(4), 278-282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2019.01.018

  • Kidd, L. R., Garrard, G. E., Bekessy, S. A., Mills, M., Camilleri, A. R., Fidler, F., Fielding, K. S., Gordon, A., Gregg, E. A., Kusmanoff, A. M., & Louis, W. (2019). Messaging matters: A systematic review of the conservation messaging literature. Biological Conservation, 236, 92-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2019.05.020

  • Kohls, L. R. (1984). The values Americans live by. Meridian House International.

  • Kolandai‐Matchett, K., & Armoudian, M. (2020). Message framing strategies for effective marine conservation communication. Aquatic Conservation, 30(12), 2441-2463. https://doi.org/10.1002/aqc.3349

  • Kollmuss, A., & Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental Education Research, 8(3), 239-260. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504620220145401

  • Kristofferson, K., White, K., & Peloza, J. (2014). The nature of slacktivism: How the social observability of an initial act of token support affects subsequent prosocial action. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(6), 1149-1166. https://doi.org/10.1086/674137

  • Kubo, T., Shoji, Y., Tsuge, T., & Kuriyama, K. (2018). Voluntary contributions to hiking trail maintenance: Evidence from a field experiment in a national park, Japan. Ecological Economics, 144, 124-128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2017.07.032

  • Kubo, T., Yokoo, H. F., & Veríssimo, D. (2023). Conservation fundraising: Evidence from social media and traditional mail field experiments. Conservation Letters, 16(1), Article e12931. https://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12931

  • Kusmanoff, A. (2017). Framing the conservation conversation: An investigation into framing techniques for communicating biodiversity conservation [Doctoral dissertation, Royal Melbourne Institute of Technology University]. RMIT University Repository. https://researchrepository.rmit.edu.au/esploro/outputs/9921863894901341

  • Lacetera, N., Macis, M., & Mele, A. (2016). Viral altruism? Charitable giving and social contagion in online networks. Sociological Science, 3(11), 202-238. https://doi.org/10.15195/v3.a11

  • Lehman, P. K., & Geller, E. S. (2004). Behavior analysis and environmental protection: Accomplishments and potential for more. Behavior and Social Issues, 13(1), 13-33. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v13i1.33

  • Li, N., & Su, L. Y. F. (2018). Message framing and climate change communication: A meta-analytical review. Journal of Applied Communications, 102(3), Article 4. https://doi.org/10.4148/1051-0834.2189

  • Li, Y., Yang, D., & Liu, Y. (2021). The effect of message framing on consumers’ intentions to purchase recycling-aiding products in China. Sustainability, 13(12), Article 6966. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13126966

  • List, J. A., & Lucking-Reiley, D. (2002). The effects of seed money and refunds on charitable giving: Experimental evidence from a university capital campaign. Journal of Political Economy, 110(1), 215-233. https://doi.org/10.1086/324392

  • MacFarlane, D., Hurlstone, M. J., Ecker, U. K. H., Ferraro, P. J., van der Linden, S., Wan, A. K. Y., Veríssimo, D., Burgess, G., Chen, F., Hall, W., Hollands, G. J., & Sutherland, W. J. (2022). Reducing demand for overexploited wildlife products: Lessons from systematic reviews from outside conservation science. Conservation Science and Practice, 4(3), Article e627. https://doi.org/10.1111/csp2.627

  • Maheswaran, D., & Meyers-Levy, J. (1990). The influence of message framing and issue involvement. Journal of Marketing Research, 27(3), 361-367. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224379002700310

  • Mangiafico, S. (2022). rcompanion: Functions to support extension education program evaluation: R package version 2.4.18. R Project. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rcompanion

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224-253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

  • Maze, K., Barnett, M., Guenther, L., Botts, E. A., Stephens, A., & Freedman, M. (2016). Making the case for biodiversity in South Africa: Re-framing biodiversity communications. Bothalia-African Biodiversity & Conservation, 46(1), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.4102/abc.v46i1.2039

  • McAfee, D., Doubleday, Z. A., Geiger, N., & Connell, S. D. (2019). Everyone loves a success story: Optimism inspires conservation engagement. Bioscience, 69(4), 274-281. https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biz019

  • Meyers-Levy, J., & Maheswaran, D. (2004). Exploring message framing outcomes when systematic, heuristic, or both types of processing occur. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(1–2), 159-167. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp1401&2_18

  • Mueller, A., & Maes, J. (2015). Arguments for biodiversity conservation in Natura 2000 sites: An analysis based on LIFE projects. Nature Conservation, 12, 1-26. https://doi.org/10.3897/natureconservation.12.4848

  • Nelson, K. M., Anggraini, E., & Schlüter, A. (2020). Virtual reality as a tool for environmental conservation and fundraising. PLoS One, 15(4), Article e0223631. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223631

  • Nelson, K. M., Bauer, M. K., & Partelow, S. (2021). Informational nudges to encourage pro-environmental behavior: Examining differences in message framing and human interaction. Frontiers in Communication, 5, Article 610186. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2020.610186

  • Nordhaus, T., & Shellenberger, M. (2009, November 16). Apocalypse fatigue: Losing the public on climate change. Yale Environment 360. https://e360.yale.edu/features/apocalypse_fatigue_losing_the_public_on_climate_change

  • Özgen, Ö., Atrek, B., Kurt, S. D., & Madran, C. (2015, June 10–13). Message framing of Facebook posts: An analysis of non-governmental organizations (NGOs). 2nd International Social Business@Anadolu Conference, Anadolu University, Eskişehir, Turkey (pp. 161–171). http://www.pazarlama.org.tr/upk-2015/konferans_dosyalari/bildiriler/89.pdf

  • Pascual, U., Balvanera, P., Christie, M., Baptiste, B., González-Jiménez, D., Anderson, C. B., Athayde, S., Chaplin-Kramer, R., Jacobs, S., Kelemen, E., Kumar, R., Lazos, E., Martin, A., Mwampamba, T. H., Nakangu, B., O’Farrell, P., Raymond, C. M., Subramanian, S. M., Termansen, M., … Vatn, A. (2022). Summary for policymakers of the methodological assessment of the diverse values and valuation of nature of the Intergovernmental Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services. IPBES Secretariat.https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6522392

  • Pelletier, L. G., & Sharp, E. (2008). Persuasive communication and proenvironmental behaviors: How message tailoring and message framing can improve the integration of behaviors through self-determined motivation. Canadian Psychology, 49(3), 210-217. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012755

  • Posit Team. (2022). RStudio: Integrated development environment for R. Posit Software/PBC. http://www.posit.co/

  • R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/

  • Ramachandra, T. V., & Bharath, S. (2020). Carbon sequestration potential of the forest ecosystems in the Western Ghats, a global biodiversity hotspot. Natural Resources Research, 29(4), 2753-2771. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11053-019-09588-0

  • Reddy, S. M., Montambault, J., Masuda, Y. J., Keenan, E., Butler, W., Fisher, J. R., Asah, S. T., & Gneezy, A. (2017). Advancing conservation by understanding and influencing human behavior. Conservation Letters, 10(2), 248-256. https://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12252

  • Reddy, S. M., Wardropper, C., Weigel, C., Masuda, Y. J., Harden, S., Ranjan, P., Getson, J. M., Esman, L. A., Ferraro, P., & Prokopy, L. (2020). Conservation behavior and effects of economic and environmental message frames. Conservation Letters, 13(6), Article e12750. https://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12750

  • Redford, K. H., & Adams, W. M. (2009). Payment for ecosystem services and the challenge of saving nature. Conservation Biology, 23(4), 785-787. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1739.2009.01271.x

  • Riemer, H., Shavitt, S., Koo, M., & Markus, H. R. (2014). Preferences don’t have to be personal: Expanding attitude theorizing with a cross-cultural perspective. Psychological Review, 121(4), 619-648. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037666

  • Ropret Homar, A. R., & Cvelbar, L. K. (2021). The effects of framing on environmental decisions: A systematic literature review. Ecological Economics, 183, Article 106950. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2021.106950

  • Salazar, G., Monroe, M. C., Ennes, M., Jones, J. A., & Veríssimo, D. (2022). Testing the influence of visual framing on engagement and pro‐environmental action. Conservation Science and Practice, 4(10), Article e12812. https://doi.org/10.1111/csp2.12812

  • Sandbrook, C., Fisher, J. A., Holmes, G., Luque-Lora, R., & Keane, A. (2019). The global conservation movement is diverse but not divided. Nature Sustainability, 2(4), 316-323. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41893-019-0267-5

  • Scannell, L., & Gifford, R. (2013). Personally relevant climate change: The role of place attachment and local versus global message framing in engagement. Environment and Behavior, 45(1), 60-85. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916511421196

  • Schmidt, S., & Eisend, M. (2015). Advertising repetition: A meta-analysis on effective frequency in advertising. Journal of Advertising, 44(4), 415-428. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2015.1018460

  • Schultz, P. W. (2002). Knowledge, information, and household recycling: Examining the knowledge-deficit model of behavior change. In T. Dietz & P. C. Stern (Eds.), New tools for environmental protection: Education, information and voluntary measures (pp. 67–82). National Academy Press.

  • Schultz, P. W. (2011). Conservation means behavior. Conservation Biology, 25(6), 1080-1083. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1739.2011.01766.x

  • Schwartz, D., Bruine de Bruin, W., Fischhoff, B., & Lave, L. (2015). Advertising energy saving programs: The potential environmental cost of emphasizing monetary savings. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 21(2), 158-166. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000042

  • Schultz, P. W., & Zelezny, L. (2003). Reframing environmental messages to be congruent with American values. Human Ecology Review, 10(2), 126-136. http://www.jstor.org/stable/24706963

  • Segev, S., Fernandes, J., & Wang, W. (2015). The effects of gain versus loss message framing and point of reference on consumer responses to green advertising. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 36(1), 35-51. https://doi.org/10.1080/10641734.2014.912600

  • Seip, K., & Strand, J. (1992). Willingness to pay for environmental goods in Norway: A contingent valuation study with real payment. Environmental and Resource Economics, 2(1), 91-106. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00324691

  • Shreedhar, G. (2021). Evaluating the impact of storytelling in Facebook advertisements on wildlife conservation engagement: Lessons and challenges. Conservation Science and Practice, 3(11), Article e534. https://doi.org/10.1111/csp2.534

  • Spence, A., & Pidgeon, N. (2010). Framing and communicating climate change: The effects of distance and outcome frame manipulations. Global Environmental Change, 20(4), 656-667. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2010.07.002

  • Sreekumar, V. B., Sreejith, K. A., Hareesh, V. S., & Sanil, M. S. (2020). An overview of wild edible fruits of Western Ghats, India. Genetic Resources and Crop Evolution, 67(7), 1659-1693. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10722-020-00986-5

  • Stadlthanner, K. A., Andreu, L., Ribeiro, M. A., Font, X., & Mattila, A. S. (2022). The effects of message framing in CSR advertising on consumers’ emotions, attitudes, and behavioral intentions. Journal of Hospitality Marketing & Management, 31(7), 777-796. https://doi.org/10.1080/19368623.2022.2065399

  • Steinhorst, J., Klöckner, C. A., & Matthies, E. (2015). Saving electricity–For the money or the environment? Risks of limiting pro-environmental spillover when using monetary framing. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 43, 125-135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.05.012

  • Szaszi, B., Higney, A., Charlton, A., Gelman, A., Ziano, I., Aczel, B., Goldstein, D. G., Yeager, D. S., & Tipton, E. (2022). No reason to expect large and consistent effects of nudge interventions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 119(31), Article e2200732119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2200732119

  • Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 67-85. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.67

  • Thompson, A. W., Reimer, A., & Prokopy, L. S. (2015). Farmers’ views of the environment: The influence of competing attitude frames on landscape conservation efforts. Agriculture and Human Values, 32(3), 385-399. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10460-014-9555-x

  • Toomey, A. H., Knight, A. T., & Barlow, J. (2017). Navigating the space between research and implementation in conservation. Conservation Letters, 10(5), 619-625. https://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12315

  • Triandis, H. C. (1996). The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. American Psychologist, 51(4), 407-415. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.51.4.407

  • Tsai, S. P. (2007). Message framing strategy for brand communication. Journal of Advertising Research, 47(3), 364-377. https://doi.org/10.2501/S0021849907070377

  • Uggla, Y. (2018). Framing and visualising biodiversity in EU policy. Journal of Integrative Environmental Sciences, 15(1), 99-118. https://doi.org/10.1080/1943815X.2018.1455714

  • van der Linden, S. (2015). Intrinsic motivation and pro-environmental behaviour. Nature Climate Change, 5(7), 612-613. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2669

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsic versus extrinsic goal contents in self-determination theory: Another look at the quality of academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 41(1), 19-31. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4101_4

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learning, performance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2), 246-260. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.246

  • Vaske, J. J., Landon, A. C., & Miller, C. A. (2020). Normative influences on farmers’ intentions to practice conservation without compensation. Environmental Management, 66(2), 191-201. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00267-020-01306-4

  • Vu, H. T., Liu, Y., & Tran, D. V. (2019). Nationalizing a global phenomenon: A study of how the press in 45 countries and territories portrays climate change. Global Environmental Change, 58, Article 101942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2019.101942

  • Walker, J. M., Godley, B. J., & Nuno, A. (2019). Media framing of the Cayman Turtle Farm: Implications for conservation conflicts. Journal for Nature Conservation, 48, 61-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jnc.2019.01.001

  • White, K., Habib, R., & Hardisty, D. J. (2019). How to SHIFT consumer behaviors to be more sustainable: A literature review and guiding framework. Journal of Marketing, 83(3), 22-49. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022242919825649

  • Wiencierz, C., Pöppel, K. G., & Röttger, U. (2015). Where does my money go? How online comments on a donation campaign influence the perceived trustworthiness of a nonprofit organization. International Journal of Strategic Communication, 9(2), 102-117. https://doi.org/10.1080/1553118X.2015.1008634

  • Xiao, A., Huang, Y., Bortree, D. S., & Waters, R. D. (2022). Designing social media fundraising messages: An experimental approach to understanding how message concreteness and framing influence donation intentions. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 51(4), 832-856. https://doi.org/10.1177/08997640211022838

  • Yoganathan, V., Osburg, V. S., & Stevens, C. J. (2021). Freedom and giving in game streams: A Foucauldian exploration of tips and donations on Twitch. Psychology and Marketing, 38(6), 1001-1013. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21483

  • Zhu, L., He, Y., Chen, Q., & Hu, M. (2017). It’s the thought that counts: The effects of construal level priming and donation proximity on consumer response to donation framing. Journal of Business Research, 76, 44-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.03.007

  • Zubair, M., Iqbal, S., Usman, S. M., Awais, M., Wang, R., & Wang, X. (2020). Message framing and self-conscious emotions help to understand pro-environment consumer purchase intention: An ERP study. Scientific Reports, 10, Article 18304. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75343-8

Appendices

Appendix 1

Table A1

List of Environmental Non-Governmental Organisations Included for the Content Analysis, for Each Country

Country Organisation
UK Chester Zoo (North of England Zoological Society)
Royal Society for the Protection of Birds RSPB
The World Parrot Trust
Twycross Zoo (East Midland Zoological Society)
World Wide Fund for Nature (WWF UK)
USA Center for Biodiversity and Conservation (American Museum of Natural History, New York)
San Diego Zoo Global
The Nature Conservancy
World Surf League PURE
World Wildlife Fund (WWF US)
South Africa BirdLife South Africa
Endangered Wildlife Trust
Southern African Foundation for the Conservation of Coastal Birds
Wildlife ACT Fund Trust
World Wide Fund for Nature (WWF South Africa)
India Balipara Tract & Frontier Foundation
Bombay Natural History Society
Wildlife Conservation Trust
Wildlife Trust of India
World Wide Fund for Nature (WWF India)
Brazil Fundação o Boticário de Proteção à Natureza
Instituto Conservation International do Brasil
Instituto de Desenvolvimento Sustentável Mamirauá
Instituto de Pesquisas Ecológicas
World Wide Fund for Nature (WWF Brasil)

Appendix 2

Table A2

The Total Usage of Message Frame Styles Across the Five Countries and All Countries Combined

Message Frame Styles UK USA South Africa India Brazil All Countries
Extrinsic 219 185 230 203 256 1,093
Intrinsic 198 151 270 183 179 981
Negative 23 80 50 57 28 238
Positive 394 256 450 329 407 1,836
Extrinsic Negative 10 33 27 27 18 115
Extrinsic Positive 209 152 203 176 238 978
Intrinsic Negative 13 47 23 30 10 123
Intrinsic Positive 185 104 247 153 169 858

Appendix 3

Click to enlarge
gep.11181-fa1
Figure A1

Usage of the Four Message Frame Styles (‘Extrinsic Negative’, ‘Extrinsic Positive’ ‘Intrinsic Negative’, ‘Intrinsic Positive’) Across the Five Countries

Note. Usage is measured as a percentage.

Appendix 4

Table A3

Total Frequencies for Social Actions Received for Each of the Four Advertisements Across the Five Countries

Message Frame Styles Country Post Likes Post Comments Post Shares Post saves
Extrinsic Negative Brazil 188 7 14 3
India 1,078 6 10 4
South Africa 111 2 2 2
UK 23 0 3 1
USA 34 5 6 1
Extrinsic Positive Brazil 191 3 26 4
India 1,447 8 6 9
South Africa 102 4 3 5
UK 30 2 0 1
USA 25 0 1 2
Intrinsic Negative Brazil 203 6 11 2
India 909 6 9 3
South Africa 87 4 2 5
UK 29 3 2 1
USA 70 4 4 2
Intrinsic Positive Brazil 220 5 15 5
India 783 6 9 3
South Africa 98 1 4 3
UK 31 1 5 1
USA 60 3 7 2

Appendix 5

Table A4

Total Number of Clicks to Website and Impressions Made for Each of the Message Frame Styles Across the Five Countries

Message Frame Styles Countries Clicks to website Impressions
Extrinsic Brazil 23,476 1,006,906
India 129,844 3,794,878
South Africa 15,056 646,463
UK 2,048 279,375
USA 2,069 171,717
Intrinsic Brazil 20,685 1,006,147
India 142,716 3,771,503
South Africa 14,912 639,523
UK 2,042 288,576
USA 2,120 163,196
Negative Brazil 21,331 1,013,359
India 131,869 4,086,948
South Africa 15,379 629,464
UK 2,039 279,613
USA 2,144 173,394
Positive Brazil 22,830 999,694
India 140,691 3,479,433
South Africa 14,589 656,522
UK 2,051 288,338
USA 2,045 161,519
Extrinsic Negative Brazil 11,338 503,547
India 66,222 2,164,877
South Africa 7,754 350,366
UK 1,076 141,257
USA 1,071 89,654
Extrinsic Positive Brazil 12,138 503,359
India 63,622 1,630,001
South Africa 7,302 296,097
UK 972 138,118
USA 998 82,063
Intrinsic Negative Brazil 9,993 509,812
India 65,647 1,922,071
South Africa 7,625 279,098
UK 963 138,356
USA 1,073 83,740
Intrinsic Positive Brazil 10,692 496,335
India 77,069 1,849,432
South Africa 7,287 360,425
UK 1,079 150,220
USA 1,047 79,456