Perspective Articles

Why Do (Or Don’t) People Protect Nature? Insights From Conservation Practice and Environmental Psychology to Respond to the Biodiversity Crisis

Lily M. van Eeden1,2,3,*, Hugh P. Possingham4, Taciano L. Milfont5,6, Christoph Klebl7, Kelly Fielding8

Global Environmental Psychology, 2025, Vol. 3, Article e10927, https://doi.org/10.5964/gep.10927

Received: 2022-12-28. Accepted: 2023-09-28. Published (VoR): 2025-02-21.

Handling Editor: John Jamir Benzon Aruta, De La Salle University, Manila, Philippines

*Corresponding author at: Building 3, RMIT University, La Trobe Street, Melbourne VIC 3000, Australia. E-mail: lily.van.eeden@rmit.edu.au

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Understanding and shaping human action towards nature conservation is critical to reversing the biodiversity crisis. Psychological science provides tools for understanding individual and collective behaviours, but also for understanding how the behaviour of individuals can drive human–environment systems transitions. As researchers and practitioners spanning distinct disciplines, we draw on our collective knowledge in environmental psychology, systems thinking, economics, and conservation biology, along with experience in practice and government, to consider reasons why people do (or don’t) protect nature. We outline dimensions important to fostering individual conservation behaviour and systems transformation. Such individual dimensions include values, personality traits, and psychological distancing. Broader system influences include cultural, economic, and environmental factors that shape the way people interact with, and care for, nature. Finally, we describe potential tools that may support increasing conservation actions and systems transformation, including strengthening connection with and access to nature, values-based and solutions-focused framing, collective action, and propagating optimism.

Keywords: environmental values, individual behaviour change, multilevel systems thinking, nature conservation, psychological distance

Non-Technical Summary

Background

The Earth’s biodiversity is being threatened by human actions. This is a complex problem to solve, requiring an understanding of both nature conservation science and the factors that drive human behaviours. The latter should include consideration of both psychology and the economic, cultural, and structural factors that shape the choices and actions people make.

Why was this study done?

Responding to this need requires interdisciplinary collaboration. However, opportunities to share insights across disciplines on a topic of interest can be limited. Doing so can support identification of important concepts and future research opportunities for developing new, more effective solutions.

What did the researchers do and find?

The authors include experts in nature conservation and environmental psychology, with experience in academia, government, and non-government roles. We came together to discuss our experiences and expertise relevant to conserving nature through human behaviour. We recorded a seminar and panel discussion, and then prepared this manuscript to describe the themes discussed. We focused on aspects of psychology and contextual (systems) influences that shape whether (or not) people protect nature. Based on these, we propose tools to promote action for nature conservation including strengthening public connection with and access to nature, values-based and solutions-focused framing, collective action, and propagating optimism. We highlighted that psychological science is important for exploring how to catalyse major systems transformations that address drivers of the biodiversity crisis.

What do these findings mean?

Our discussion demonstrates that bringing together researchers and practitioners from diverse disciplines is a valuable practice for identifying and prioritising research questions and interventions. The themes and tools we discuss provide a platform for future interdisciplinary collaboration with an applied and solution-oriented focus.

Highlights

  • Meaningfully responding to the global biodiversity crisis requires complementary transdisciplinary approaches, including multilevel systems thinking and psychology.

  • Individuals are key in implementing transformative systems change, whether through advocating for change externally or through powerful individuals making bold decisions in government or industry.

  • Psychological science is important for improving connection with nature and making environmental values salient to support private and public sphere conservation behaviour.

The biodiversity crisis continues to worsen as we witness the Earth’s first human-induced mass species extinction (Ceballos et al., 2017; Cowie et al., 2022). In less than 50 years, wildlife populations have decreased by 69% and currently 28% of all assessed species are threatened with extinction (Almond et al., 2022). Over the past half century, natural sciences have tended to form the dominant, if not sole, evidence base informing conservation action, but such evidence alone is insufficient to address the crisis (Bennett et al., 2017). Understanding and influencing human behaviour is increasingly recognised as an important tool to achieve nature conservation objectives (Nilsson et al., 2020; Reddy et al., 2017; Schultz, 2011), with exploration of values and attitudes as predictors of behaviour forming a focus of conservation social science research in recent years (Nilsson et al., 2020; Reddy et al., 2017; Selinske et al., 2018). As reviewed by Bamberg and Möser (2007), existing theoretical models of pro-environmental behaviours, including conservation behaviours, can be grouped into those that view behaviour as primarily motivated by self-interest, e.g., the theory of reasoned action (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980) and associated theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, 1991), or those that view behaviour primarily as pro-socially motivated, e.g., the norm activation model (Schwartz, 1977), and the related value-belief-norm theory (Stern, 2000). We support their conclusion that conservation behaviours are influenced by a mixture of self-interest and pro-social motives, as well as by external influences. Indeed, such behaviours are not solely an individual phenomenon, but occur within complex systems of cultural, institutional, and technological influences (Milfont & Markowitz, 2016). Understanding human behaviour is complex and approached by multiple social science disciplines, with no single theory addressing the full range of factors shaping action (Eyster et al., 2022).

In fact, the biodiversity crisis is a wicked problem, requiring interdisciplinary solutions. Individual actions to protect nature might encompass a wide variety of behaviours that have a direct or indirect positive impact on nature. Likewise, abstaining from detrimental behaviours, e.g., habitat destruction, unsustainable wildlife harvesting, is similarly valuable to conservation objectives. In this paper, we consider conservation behaviours to include actions such as environmental volunteering, nature-friendly gardening practices, conservation advocacy, or sustainable consumption that may have a direct or indirect impact on nature (Selinske et al., 2020). We are particularly interested in behaviours that may be relevant and accessible to the majority populace, as opposed to industry- or other context-specific behaviours that may have a high impact but pertain to a small target audience. A focus on individual behaviours has been criticised as inadequate for addressing societal issues, and yet individual-level research remains crucial to successful systems-thinking approaches to implementing transformative change (Chater & Loewenstein, 2022; Klebl & Jetten, 2023). Understanding individual behaviour is valuable not only because of the impacts that individuals can make through private-sphere conservation behaviours, but also because individuals can initiate structural change, including via advocacy (Stern, 2000). Decisions to implement systems change approaches are implemented by powerful individuals such as those in industry or government, so it is important to understand why those individuals might (or might not) take bold actions for nature conservation. Systems-thinking entails a “big picture”, interdisciplinary approach to explore dynamic relationships between different elements shaping complex conservation issues, as opposed to breaking down issues into discrete elements to be addressed separately (Abson et al., 2017). Systems approaches support identifying barriers and leverage points to promote systems transitions, allowing interventions to have impact beyond individual behaviour change approaches (Abson et al., 2017). As such, understanding how systems transformation can occur to improve conservation outcomes can benefit from both broad systems thinking and individual psychology (Chater & Loewenstein, 2022).

In this paper, we explore the factors influencing whether people want to protect nature by drawing on the experience and knowledge of: (1) an eminent ecologist and conservationist with a wealth of experience in academic research, conservation practice, and policy development (HP), (2) a leading academic environmental psychologist who now applies behavioural insights in government (TM), and (3) a government-based researcher drawing together conservation biology and human behaviour change science to achieve nature conservation objectives (LV) following a seminar and panel discussion between these authors on 11 November 2022 (Psychology of Change, 2023). The seminar was chaired by a leading environmental psychologist (KF) with further contributions from an early career social psychologist (CK). Collaboration between natural and social scientists is critical to inform robust responses to complex interdisciplinary questions underpinning the biodiversity crisis (Martin, 2020). Further, collaboration between academics and practitioners can support applied research and translation into action (Kates et al., 2001; Lang et al., 2012). Together, we have developed a list of key factors shaping conservation behaviours at multiple levels and consider opportunities to address them to meaningfully respond to the biodiversity crisis. All authors currently live and work in Western, majority English-speaking locations. As such, our discussion pertains mostly to similar contexts, although we seek to draw on more diverse, global examples where possible.

Who Protects Nature, and Why?

Whether innate or learned, humans are fascinated by nature (Simaika & Samways, 2010). The ‘biophilia’ hypothesis purports that humans have a natural affinity for other living things and that this affinity has had evolutionary benefits (Kellert & Wilson, 1993). Contrasting with ‘biophilia’ is ‘biophobia’, defined as our innate fear of aspects of the natural world which may threaten us (Simaika & Samways, 2010). ‘Biophobia’ reminds us that not all nature experiences are positive; rather some may elicit feelings of fear, disgust, or discomfort (von Döhren & Haase, 2015). From a historical perspective, many highly urbanised societies may have reduced their interactions and connection with nature (Richardson et al., 2022; Soga & Gaston, 2016). This is purported to be linked with cultural views (particularly those cultures rooted in Judeo-Christian religious origins) of humans as separate from nature (Manfredo et al., 2016; Newman & Dale, 2013), and even when people hold affinity for nature, this does not necessarily result in conservation behaviour (Amel et al., 2017; Gifford, 2011). This is coupled with low levels of ecoliteracy. An ability to name species can be regarded as a foundational form of familiarity with one’s ecosystem (Mikołajczak et al., 2021) and many people in urban places are unable to identify or perceive common urban wildlife species (e.g., in Chile, China, the Netherlands, and the Scandinavian countries; Celis-Diez et al., 2017; Hooykaas et al., 2019; Kai et al., 2014; Palmberg et al., 2015). This is an issue because learning about nature can promote connection with nature (Ng et al., 2023; Palmberg et al., 2015), and connection with nature is recognised as a predictor of pro-conservation behaviours (Whitburn et al., 2020).

As many conservationists will attest, formative experiences and connection with nature during childhood can be a pathway towards conservation action in adulthood (Chawla, 1999, 2020; Pinder et al., 2020; Soga & Gaston, 2023). There is also evidence indicating that connection with nature is strong in childhood but reduces during adolescence (Keith et al., 2021) and then is typically highest late in life (Dean et al., 2018). Creators of children’s entertainment tap into our early fascination with nature, although portrayal of nature in children’s books may be decreasing over time (Babb et al., 2018). Some scholars attribute disconnection from nature to an ‘extinction of experience’, a perceived phenomenon whereby humans have reduced their direct interactions with nature due to declining opportunities for experiential nature connection as a result of increasing urbanisation (e.g., Colléony et al., 2020; Gaston & Soga, 2020; Pyle, 2003). However, the existence of this ‘extinction of experience’ is debated. Highly urbanised populations do have opportunity to experience nature in cities, albeit different (perhaps less immersive, rich, and diverse) experiences compared to those in more remote areas (Newman & Dale, 2013). Evidence suggests that urbanites have not lost their emotional connection with nature (Oh et al., 2020) and that pro-conservation attitudes are predicted by family biospheric values and environmental volunteering rather than experiences in nature (Pinder et al., 2020). This implies that conservation behaviour is influenced by socialisation and shared values, but the role of direct nature interaction in predicting conservation behaviour remains contested. If our relationships with local, urban nature shape our concern and action for nature more broadly, including voting, advocacy, donations, and consumption choices, then urban nature relationships may shape the fate of nature globally (Dunn et al., 2006).

Lifestyle and demographic factors can also be important in shaping nature connection and action. Women are more likely to associate with nature conservation than men (Zelezny et al., 2000). Stronger connection with nature late in life (Dean et al., 2018) might suggest that slowing down or having the freedom from work (e.g., retiring) could facilitate nature connection. Yet, people who engage in naturalist activities such as bird watching, citizen science, or wildlife tourism can be diverse in their motivations for engagement, level of commitment to the activity, and demographics (Steven et al., 2021). There has not yet been a global profile review of hobby naturalists, but a general trend appears to encompass higher levels of education and wealth, and contributors to citizen science are predominantly male and/or white (Blake et al., 2020; Cong et al., 2017; Cooper et al., 2021; Pateman et al., 2021). While naturalist activities have previously tended towards older people (Connell, 2009; Green & Jones, 2010), new technology and approaches, such as app-based citizen science and gamification, seek to make natural history learning more appealing and accessible to young people (Herodotou et al., 2023; Newman et al., 2012).

Wealth seems to be a predictor of pro-conservation behaviour at a population and individual level (Milfont & Markowitz, 2016). In urban centres, wealthier areas typically have more urban green space (e.g., in Australia, China, the United States; Astell-Burt et al., 2014; Schell et al., 2020; Shanahan et al., 2014; Wolch et al., 2014), meaning disadvantaged urban communities have less access to nature. Adding to this disparity is evidence indicating that spending time in nature can have mental and physical health benefits (Martin et al., 2020). Links between nature connection, socio-economic status, and wellbeing may suggest that efforts to engage urban communities in nature could result in multiple ‘wins’, benefiting social and health objectives, while also fostering connection with nature and potentially greater adoption of conservation behaviours.

A range of individual characteristics can predict engagement (or lack of) in conservation, and these may be acquired or inherited (Milfont, 2021). Important characteristics include (1) personality traits, such as agreeableness, and honesty or humility (Soutter et al., 2020); (2) future orientation, i.e., those who think about the future consequences of their current actions (such as associated with having children, Milfont et al., 2012; Shrum et al., 2023); (3) conservative system-justifying ideologies, such as political conservatism, social dominance orientation, and right-wing authoritarianism that oppose increased environmental action (Stanley et al., 2019); and (4) basic values, particularly high levels of self-transcendence and openness to change (Milfont, 2021).

There is a vast literature on environmental values that describe how and why one might (or might not) appreciate attributes of nature. Value frameworks help us to understand differences in the beliefs, attitudes, and actions people have or make towards nature, although any one person may hold a diversity of values relevant to nature conservation that become variably salient in different contexts. Some of the overarching themes include utilitarian or instrumental values (nature is valuable for humans to extract from), intrinsic values (nature is valuable in its own right, even if humans don’t benefit from it), and relational values (nature is valuable because of the relationships it helps me to have with others, the cultural identity or sense of place it provides me, or the feelings of stewardship I enjoy from caring for it) (Chan et al., 2016). Additionally, altruistic and biospheric values may promote conservation behaviours, whereas egoistic values may be more commonly associated with anti-conservation behaviours (de Groot & Steg, 2008; Schultz & Zelezny, 1998). The Wildlife Value Orientations framework proposes that people will support or engage in extractive wildlife behaviours (fishing, hunting) or culling (which may or may not be beneficial to conservation depending on context) if they hold stronger utilitarian values as opposed to mutualistic values, which emphasise coexistence with wildlife and prioritise prevention of cruelty to animals (Fulton et al., 1996). Conversely, utilitarian and egoistic behaviours may also be conducive to conservation behaviours if one perceives that a healthy environment provides material or economic value or will support good quality of life (Díaz et al., 2015; Pascual et al., 2017). Good quality of life is not solely tied to economic or material benefit, and people may perceive of intrinsic value and relational values in their relationships with nature that motivate them to act in pro-nature ways (Díaz et al., 2015).

Entailed in our relationships with nature are affective dimensions that shape behaviours, and pleasant feelings of awe and fascination can promote pro-conservation and pro-social behaviour (Ibanez et al., 2017; Song et al., 2023; Yang et al., 2018). Values can also influence such affective dimensions, shaping perceptions of risk associated with nature (Landon et al., 2020) and empathy towards non-human animals, which may shape behaviour (Manfredo et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2023). In describing “biophilia”, Kellert and Wilson (1993) proposed that some people (e.g., ecologists) hold ecologistic-scientific values, receiving fulfilment from documenting and understanding nature and the relationships between organisms.

Conservation issues also vary in their psychological salience in different contexts and for different individuals, e.g., depending on factors such as diverse levels of media coverage, whether a given issue was recently or commonly encountered, or the perception of risk to an individual or group (Clayton & Myers, 2009). Such factors, e.g., media coverage and social discussion, may mean that climate change has become the focus of much of the conversation about environmental issues, sidelining biodiversity conservation (Boscarino, 2015; Veríssimo et al., 2014). While climate change and the biodiversity crisis may be linked, they are not the same thing and climate change is not currently the primary threat to species and ecosystems (rather, land use change is, Caro et al., 2022). This means that actions to address these two crises may not be the same. Those actions that benefit biodiversity but are of low priority for climate change mitigation may be devalued by target audiences and institutions seeking to change behaviour.

Psychological distance describes how people perceive that something is close or far from them (Trope et al., 2007; Trope & Liberman, 2010). It can manifest in four related dimensions: perceptions that environmental problems are too uncertain (hypotheticality dimension); they will occur at a distant time in the future (temporal dimension); they will occur far away geographically (spatial dimension); and they will affect someone other than myself (social dimension) (Milfont, 2010). These perceptions are forms of self-serving bias as people seek to protect their perception of their valued place(s) by underestimating environmental risks and degradation, and it is related to people’s perception to view themselves as possessing more positive qualities than average (Schultz et al., 2014). The spatial dimension is sometimes referred to as “environmental hyperopia” or “spatial optimism” (Gifford et al., 2009; Uzzell, 2000) and describes how people perceive global environmental problems to be worse than local environmental problems (Milfont & Thomson, 2020; Schultz et al., 2014). Conversely, personal experiences of local weather anomalies appear to promote perception of climate change as a risk, motivating political engagement (Egan & Mullin, 2012; Myers et al., 2013). Related to the temporal dimension is the concept of “shifting baselines”, whereby individuals set their perceptions from their own experience, failing to pass on this experience to future generations, and current generations fail to appreciate the experiences of previous generations (Moore et al., 2019; Papworth et al., 2009; Soga & Gaston, 2018). As each generation inherits new norms about the natural environment, their acceptable thresholds for environmental conditions are continually lowered and they may fail to recognise that degradation is occurring (Moore et al., 2019; Soga & Gaston, 2018).

Psychological distance and other such biases could result in lower propensity to engage in conservation behaviours that might otherwise be motivated by environmental concern. However, whether higher psychological distance relating to climate change can be linked with lower propensity to engage in mitigating or adaptive behaviours, is context-specific, in part because psychological distance is multi-faceted and may be mediated or moderated by a range of other factors (see Keller et al., 2022 for a review). Nature connection literature proposes that increased contact with local nature is conducive to acting in conservation ways, but we are not aware of any studies specifically exploring the relationship between the psychological distance of nature degradation and propensity to engage in conservation behaviours.

Informing Tools to Promote Action for Nature

In our view, to have meaningful impact on nature conservation, human-centred strategies need to incorporate both psychological science and systems-focused approaches drawing on multiple disciplines. We advocate seeking to foster societies’ connection with nature by increasing and improving equality of access to nature, providing people with opportunities to engage in easy and non-threatening activities (e.g., bird watching, walking in local nature areas). As scholars continue to explore relationships between direct nature interaction, urbanisation, and conservation behaviour (e.g., Martin et al., 2020; Richardson et al., 2020), systems-level approaches should be implemented seeking to improve connection with and access to nature, while also supporting conservation outcomes. Nature-based urban design can support increasing biodiversity in cities while also increasing positive human-nature interactions (Garrard et al., 2018). Nature-based prescribing to address non-communicable health problems can also have social benefit and build public recognition of the value of healthy natural environments (Robinson & Breed, 2019). Integrating nature literacy and interaction in school curricula can foster nature connection (Harvey et al., 2020). These support conservation behaviour and support for conservation action through increasing nature connection (Richardson et al., 2020), and by making pro-environmental values salient and increasing positive attitudes towards the value of healthy environments (Homer & Kahle, 1988; Milfont et al., 2010). All these approaches would benefit from collaboration between conservation experts and other disciplines and practices (e.g., urban planning, health, education) (Butt & Dimitrijević, 2022; Keitsch & Vermeulen, 2021; Klein, 2020).

Leveraging values provides an opportunity to improve implementation of behaviour change campaigns or raise support for conservation action. While societal values may be very slow to change, communication can be made more effective by framing around salient values, beliefs, and attitudes relevant to the target audience and behaviour (Kidd, Garrard et al., 2019; Manfredo et al., 2017; Schultz & Zelezny, 2003). In targeting people who typically oppose or are disengaged from conservation action, practitioners should frame their conservation message based on values that are important to the particular group (Wolsko et al., 2016; but see Kim et al., 2023). For example, messages that frame conservation action as beneficial to values associated with conservatism such as family, tradition, patriotism, and individual rights may increase support among conservative target audiences (Schultz & Zelezny, 2003; Wolsko et al., 2016). Moreover, framing about species extinctions targeting those holding utilitarian or self-serving values might highlight that we depend on such species for our survival.

Behaviour change campaigns employing messaging can leverage social identity. The social identity approach proposes that individuals ascribe to groups to bolster self-identity, providing social meaning and reducing uncertainty about appropriate attitudes, norms, and behaviours (Hogg, 2000; Hornsey, 2008; Tajfel, 1982). Individuals form stereotypes of the ‘ideal’ members of groups that they identify with and use this stereotype as a model for how they should think and act (Tajfel, 1978). Individuals also form stereotypes of ‘out-group’ members, which typically form opposing or otherwise conflicting values with their ‘in-group’. People are more likely to be persuaded by information presented to them by in-group messengers than by people they perceive to be in their ‘out-group’ (e.g., climate change messaging, Fielding et al., 2020). Messages from in-group messengers can benefit from employing ‘social proof’, emphasising in-group social norms by highlighting that other group members are engaging in the target behaviour (Bollinger et al., 2023; Cialdini, 2006). National identity that incorporates conservation norms can also mobilise action (Milfont et al., 2020).

Psychology is also a critical part of systems transformation (Chater & Loewenstein, 2022). Behaviour change interventions targeting individuals can have outsized effects, for example, through developing social proof and changing social norms, as described above. Such interventions can engage with transforming components of systems, such as promoting advocacy for, or at least being supportive of, change in government or industry. Furthermore, shifting systems to change can involve influencing powerful individuals, such as decision makers in government, industry, or non-government organisations. Among other influences, including system attributes, the decisions and actions made by those individuals are shaped by socio-psychological factors such as values and beliefs (e.g., Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Stern, 2000). Indeed, personality traits have been identified as important predictors of whether public servants make change in their organisation (Bolton, 2022), and, like any members of society, both public servants and elected representatives vary in the extent that they are interested in and will advocate for pro-nature change. Leaders in government choose who they employ, so hiring staff who can act as effective internal advocates can be an important part of making change in the organisation. Many governments are now creating behavioural insights units (OECD, 2017) to target both public behaviour and the behaviour of their own staff. Nonetheless, progress in government is often slow given political and economic constraints, so conservation advocates recognise that the urgent change needed must happen elsewhere; for example, they might implement public land management activities without permission (Hung, 2017) or directly target industry through consumer pressure rather than waiting for government to develop a regulatory mechanism to reduce impacts to biodiversity (not without controversy, see Aldashev et al., 2015).

While individuals can influence and shape systems transformation, there are strong opposing forces impeding systems-level change (Chater & Loewenstein, 2022). There are many examples of governments failing to adequately act on or prioritise conservation issues; for example, the new Swedish government disbanded its environment ministry (Dougall, 2022) and the outgoing Brazilian government has dismantled its capacity to limit deforestation of the Amazon (Fonseca et al., 2022). In a ‘post-truth’ world, misinformation and distrust in science clouds decisions and understanding among the public, which can result in suboptimal decision making (Ecker et al., 2022; Lewandowsky et al., 2012). Industries engage in ‘greenwashing’ to continue encouraging consumption and to avoid making major changes to their practices that might affect their profits, and individuals who uphold more defensive forms of national identity (i.e., entailing narcissistic belief in superiority of one’s nation over others) are more likely to accept and support greenwashing tactics (Cichocka & Cislak, 2020; Cislak et al., 2021; Cislak et al., 2023). Pushing non-transformative solutions like greenwashing is an example of a discourse of delay that stalls or weakens action (Lamb et al., 2020).

For all these reasons, countering these narratives may be an important tool to promote action. For example, optimistic messaging may be effective in countering doomism and an emphasis on the downside of action over its benefits (Figueres & Rivett-Carnac, 2021). As it became recognised as a field, conservation biology was labelled a crisis discipline (Soulé, 1985), and negative messages tend to dominate the narrative (Kidd, Bekessy, & Garrard, 2019). However, such messages, which may incite fear or pessimism, can cause people to disengage or feel powerless to act. We can foster and benefit from “conservation optimism” by focusing on success stories and being solution-oriented (e.g., conservationoptimism.org; McAfee et al., 2019). Optimism can support feelings of efficacy, which can result in increased conservation action (Bandura, 2000). Individuals uniting with like-minded groups such as grassroots advocacy or environmental volunteering groups can support perceptions of collective effectiveness and identity as part of a group (Fielding et al., 2008; Tajfel & Turner, 1979) as well as offering the emotional support and resources advocates needed to maintain optimism and push for change that can take a long time. Getting people to connect with their local natural environments may support positive human-nature interactions that foster optimism and stewardship, improve ecoliteracy, and act as a catalyst to engaging in conservation behaviours (Richardson & Sheffield, 2017).

This manuscript results from dialogue between individuals who sit at the academic–government interface, with expertise in conservation and psychology. We recognise that we largely represent Western scientific understanding of nature and nature relationships, and that our epistemologies and concepts may have limited meaning for culturally and linguistically diverse communities (Majid et al., 2018; Sedawi et al., 2021; Taylor, 2018). For example, we have limited understanding of how concepts of nature connectedness may or may not be relevant to how Indigenous communities view their relationship with nature (Cowie et al., 2016; Sangha et al., 2019; Stoeckl et al., 2021). As conservation advocates seek to restore natural environments and societies’ relationships with them, the research and practice informing this work must be informed by interdisciplinary, inclusive collaboration that recognises such knowledge gaps. We hope this manuscript supports establishing effective partnerships to change the way conservation advocates think about solutions to the biodiversity crisis, including tackling individual behaviour within complex systems.

Openness and Transparency Statements

The present article has been checked by its handling editor(s) for compliance with the journal's open science and transparency policies. The completed Transparency Checklist is publicly available at: http://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.16054

Author Contributions

Lily M. van Eeden: Conceptualization | Project administration | Writing – original draft | Writing – review & editing.

Hugh P. Possingham: Conceptualization | Writing – review & editing.

Taciano L. Milfont: Conceptualization | Writing – review & editing.

Christoph Klebl: Conceptualization | Writing – review & editing.

Kelly Fielding: Conceptualization | Writing – review & editing.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge that the ideas presented in this paper came from a panel discussion between some of the authors (Hugh Possingham, Taciano L. Milfont, Lily van Eeden, Kelly Fielding). We are grateful to audience members who contributed insightful questions. This discussion was part of the Political and Environmental Psychology and Social Science seminar series supported by the Mary Lee bequest to the University of Queensland’s Social Change Lab.

Funding

This work was supported by the Mary Lee bequest to the University of Queensland’s Social Change Lab.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Badges for Good Research Practices

Open data: NO.

Open code: NO.

Open materials: NO.

Preregistration: NO.

Diversity statement: NO.

Note: YES = the present article meets the criteria for awarding the badge. NO = the present article does not meet the criteria for awarding the badge or the criteria are not applicable.

References

  • Abson, D. J., Fischer, J., Leventon, J., Newig, J., Schomerus, T., Vilsmaier, U., von Wehrden, H., Abernethy, P., Ives, C. D., Jager, N. W., & Lang, D. J. (2017). Leverage points for sustainability transformation. Ambio, 46(1), 30-39. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13280-016-0800-y

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behaviour. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 179-211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behaviour. Prentice-Hall

  • Aldashev, G., Limardi, M., & Verdier, T. (2015). Watchdogs of the Invisible Hand: NGO monitoring and industry equilibrium. Journal of Development Economics, 116, 28-42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2015.03.006

  • Almond, R. E. A., Grooten, M., Juffe Bignoli, D., & Petersen, T. (Eds.). (2022). Living Planet Report 2022—Building a nature-positive society. WWF.

  • Amel, E., Manning, C., Scott, B., & Koger, S. (2017). Beyond the roots of human inaction: Fostering collective effort toward ecosystem conservation. Science, 356(6335), 275-279. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aal1931

  • Astell-Burt, T., Feng, X., Mavoa, S., Badland, H. M., & Giles-Corti, B. (2014). Do low-income neighbourhoods have the least green space? A cross-sectional study of Australia’s most populous cities. BMC Public Health, 14, Article 292. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-292

  • Babb, Y. M., McBurnie, J., & Miller, K. K. (2018). Tracking the environment in Australian children’s literature: The Children’s Book Council of Australia Picture Book of the Year Awards 1955–2014. Environmental Education Research, 24(5), 716-730. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504622.2017.1326020

  • Bamberg, S., & Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27(1), 14-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.12.002

  • Bandura, A. (2000). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(3), 75-78. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00064

  • Bennett, N. J., Roth, R., Klain, S. C., Chan, K., Christie, P., Clark, D. A., Cullman, G., Curran, D., Durbin, T. J., Epstein, G., Greenberg, A., Nelson, M. P., Sandlos, J., Stedman, R., Teel, T. L., Thomas, R., Verissimo, D., & Wyborn, C. (2017). Conservation social science: Understanding and integrating human dimensions to improve conservation. Biological Conservation, 205, 93-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2016.10.006

  • Blake, C., Rhanor, A., & Pajic, C. (2020). The demographics of citizen science participation and its implications for data quality and environmental justice. Citizen Science: Theory and Practice, 5(1), Article 21. https://doi.org/10.5334/cstp.320

  • Bollinger, B., Gillingham, K. T., & Wight, K. G. (2023). Making prosocial social: The effectiveness of social proof for energy conservation using social media. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 8(3), 290-300. https://doi.org/10.1086/725031

  • Bolton, M. (2022). A system leverage points approach to governance for sustainable development. Sustainability Science, 17, 2427-2457. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-022-01188-x

  • Boscarino, J. E. (2015). Paving the way or crowding out? The impact of the rise of climate change on environmental issue agendas. Journal of Environmental Studies and Sciences, 5(2), 99-110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13412-015-0240-1

  • Butt, A. N., & Dimitrijević, B. (2022). Multidisciplinary and transdisciplinary collaboration in nature-based design of sustainable architecture and urbanism. Sustainability, 14(16), Article 10339. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141610339

  • Caro, T., Rowe, Z., Berger, J., Wholey, P., & Dobson, A. (2022). An inconvenient misconception: Climate change is not the principal driver of biodiversity loss. Conservation Letters, 15(3), Article e12868. https://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12868

  • Ceballos, G., Ehrlich, P. R., & Dirzo, R. (2017). Biological annihilation via the ongoing sixth mass extinction signaled by vertebrate population losses and declines. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), E6089-E6096. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704949114

  • Celis-Diez, J. L., Muñoz, C. E., Abades, S., Marquet, P. A., & Armesto, J. J. (2017). Biocultural homogenization in urban settings: Public knowledge of birds in city parks of Santiago, Chile. Sustainability, 9(4), Article 485. https://doi.org/10.3390/su9040485

  • Chan, K. M. A., Balvanera, P., Benessaiah, K., Chapman, M., Díaz, S., Gómez-Baggethun, E., Gould, R., Hannahs, N., Jax, K., Klain, S., Luck, G. W., Martín-López, B., Muraca, B., Norton, B., Ott, K., Pascual, U., Satterfield, T., Tadaki, M., Taggart, J., & Turner, N. (2016). Why protect nature? Rethinking values and the environment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(6), 1462-1465. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1525002113

  • Chater, N., & Loewenstein, G. (2022). The i-frame and the s-frame: How focusing on individual-level solutions has led behavioral public policy astray. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.4046264

  • Chawla, L. (1999). Life paths into effective environmental action. Journal of Environmental Education, 31(1), 15-26. https://doi.org/10.1080/00958969909598628

  • Chawla, L. (2020). Childhood nature connection and constructive hope: A review of research on connecting with nature and coping with environmental loss. People and Nature, 2(3), 619-642. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10128

  • Cialdini, R. B. (2006). Influence: The psychology of persuasion. Harper Collins.

  • Cichocka, A., & Cislak, A. (2020). Nationalism as collective narcissism. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 34, 69-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.12.013

  • Cislak, A., Cichocka, A., Wojcik, A. D., & Milfont, T. L. (2021). Words not deeds: National narcissism, national identification, and support for greenwashing versus genuine proenvironmental campaigns. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 74, Article 101576. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101576

  • Cislak, A., Wójcik, A. D., Borkowska, J., & Milfont, T. L. (2023). Secure and defensive forms of national identity and public support for climate policies. PLOS Climate, 2(6), Article e0000146.

  • Clayton, S., & Myers, G. (2009). Conservation psychology: Understanding and promoting human care for nature. John Wiley & Sons.

  • Colléony, A., Cohen-Seffer, R., & Shwartz, A. (2020). Unpacking the causes and consequences of the extinction of experience. Biological Conservation, 251, Article 108788. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2020.108788

  • Cong, L., Newsome, D., Wu, B., & Morrison, A. M. (2017). Wildlife tourism in China: A review of the Chinese research literature. Current Issues in Tourism, 20(11), 1116-1139. https://doi.org/10.1080/13683500.2014.948811

  • Connell, J. (2009). Birdwatching, twitching and tourism: Towards an Australian perspective. Australian Geographer, 40(2), 203-217. https://doi.org/10.1080/00049180902964942

  • Cooper, C. B., Hawn, C. L., Larson, L. R., Parrish, J. K., Bowser, G., Cavalier, D., Dunn, R. R., Haklay, M., Gupta, K. K., Jelks, N. T. O., Johnson, V. A., Katti, M., Leggett, Z., Wilson, O. R., & Wilson, S. (2021). Inclusion in citizen science: The conundrum of rebranding. Science, 372(6549), 1386-1388. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abi6487

  • Cowie, L. J., Greaves, L. M., Milfont, T. L., Houkamau, C. A., & Sibley, C. G. (2016). Indigenous identity and environmental values: Do spirituality and political consciousness predict environmental regard among Māori? International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, Consultation, 5(4), 228-244. https://doi.org/10.1037/ipp0000059

  • Cowie, R. H., Bouchet, P., & Fontaine, B. (2022). The Sixth Mass Extinction: Fact, fiction or speculation? Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 97(2), 640-663. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12816

  • de Groot, J. I. M., & Steg, L. (2008). Value orientations to explain beliefs related to environmental significant behavior. Environment and Behavior, 40(3), 330-354. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916506297831

  • Dean, J. H., Shanahan, D. F., Bush, R., Gaston, K. J., Lin, B. B., Barber, E., Franco, L., & Fuller, R. A. (2018). Is nature relatedness associated with better mental and physical health? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(7), Article 1371. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15071371

  • Díaz, S., Demissew, S., Carabias, J., Joly, C., Lonsdale, M., Ash, N., Larigauderie, A., Adhikari, J. R., Arico, S., Báldi, A., Bartuska, A., Baste, I. A., Bilgin, A., Brondizio, E., Chan, K. M. A., Figueroa, V. E., Duraiappah, A., Fischer, M., Hill, R., . . .Zlatanova, D. (2015). The IPBES Conceptual Framework—Connecting nature and people. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability, 14, 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2014.11.002

  • Dougall, D. M. (2022, October 18). ‘Devastating consequences’ as new Swedish government scraps environment industry. Euronews. https://www.euronews.com/2022/10/18/devastating-consequences-as-new-swedish-government-scraps-environment-ministry

  • Dunn, R. R., Gavin, M. C., Monica, C. S., & Jennifer, N. S. (2006). The pigeon paradox: Dependence of global conservation on urban nature. Conservation Biology, 20(6), 1814-1816. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1739.2006.00533.x

  • Ecker, U. K. H., Lewandowsky, S., Cook, J., Schmid, P., Fazio, L. K., Brashier, N., Kendeou, P., Vraga, E. K., & Amazeen, M. A. (2022). The psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correction. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1, 13-29. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-021-00006-y

  • Egan, P. J., & Mullin, M. (2012). Turning personal experience into political attitudes: The effect of local weather on Americans’ perceptions about global warming. Journal of Politics, 74(3), 796-809. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022381612000448

  • Eyster, H. N., Satterfield, T., & Chan, K. M. A. (2022). Why people do what they do: An interdisciplinary synthesis of human action theories. Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 47, 725-751. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-environ-020422-125351

  • Fielding, K. S., Hornsey, M. J., Thai, H. A., & Toh, L. L. (2020). Using ingroup messengers and ingroup values to promote climate change policy. Climatic Change, 158, 181-199. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-019-02561-z

  • Fielding, K. S., McDonald, R., & Louis, W. R. (2008). Theory of planned behaviour, identity and intentions to engage in environmental activism. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(4), 318-326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.03.003

  • Figueres, C., & Rivett-Carnac, T. (2021). The future we choose: The stubborn optimist's guide to the climate crisis. Vintage.

  • Fonseca, I. F. d., Lindoso, D. P., & Bursztyn, M. (2022). Deforestation (lack of) control in the Brazilian Amazon: From strengthening to dismantling governmental authority (1999–2020). Sustainability in Debate, 13(2), 12-31. https://doi.org/10.18472/SustDeb.v13n2.2022.44532

  • Fulton, D. C., Manfredo, M. J., & Lipscomb, J. (1996). Wildlife value orientations: A conceptual and measurement approach. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, 1(2), 24-47. https://doi.org/10.1080/10871209609359060

  • Garrard, G. E., Williams, N. S. G., Mata, L., Thomas, J., & Bekessy, S. A. (2018). Biodiversity sensitive urban design. Conservation Letters, 11(2), Article e12411. https://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12411

  • Gaston, K. J., & Soga, M. (2020). Extinction of experience: The need to be more specific. People and Nature, 2(3), 575-581. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10118

  • Gifford, R. (2011). The dragons of inaction: Psychological barriers that limit climate change mitigation and adaptation. American Psychologist, 66(4), 290-302. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023566

  • Gifford, R., Scannell, L., Kormos, C., Smolova, L., Biel, A., Boncu, S., Corral, V., Güntherf, H., Hanyu, K., Hine, D., Kaiser, F. G., Korpela, K., Lima, L. M., Mertig, A. G., Mira, R. G., Moser, G., Passafaro, P., Pinheiro, J. Q., Saini, S., . . .Uzzell, D. (2009). Temporal pessimism and spatial optimism in environmental assessments: An 18-nation study. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(1), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.06.001

  • Green, R. J., & Jones, D. N. (2010). Practices, needs and attitudes of bird-watching tourists in Australia. Sustainable Tourism Cooperative Research Centre.

  • Harvey, D. J., Montgomery, L. N., Harvey, H., Hall, F., Gange, A. C., & Watling, D. (2020). Psychological benefits of a biodiversity-focussed outdoor learning program for primary school children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 67, Article 101381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101381

  • Herodotou, C., Ismail, N. I.Benavides Lahnstein, A., Aristeidou, M., Young, A. N., Johnson, R. F., Higgins, L. M., Ghadiri Khanaposhtani, M., Robinson, L. D., & Ballard, H. L. (2023). Young people in iNaturalist: A blended learning framework for biodiversity monitoring. International Journal of Science Education, Part B, 14(2), 129-156.

  • Hogg, M. A. (2000). Subjective uncertainty reduction through self-categorization: A motivational theory of social identity processes. European Review of Social Psychology, 11, 223-255. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792772043000040

  • Homer, P. M., & Kahle, L. R. (1988). A structural equation test of the value-attitude-behavior hierarchy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 638-646. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.638

  • Hooykaas, M. J. D., Schilthuizen, M., Aten, C., Hemelaar, E. M., Albers, C. J., & Smeets, I. (2019). Identification skills in biodiversity professionals and laypeople: A gap in species literacy. Biological Conservation, 238, Article 108202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2019.108202

  • Hornsey, M. J. (2008). Social identity theory and self‐categorization theory: A historical review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 204-222. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00066.x

  • Hung, H. (2017). Formation of new property rights on government land through informal co-management: Case studies on countryside guerilla gardening. Land Use Policy, 63, 381-393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landusepol.2017.01.024

  • Ibanez, L., Moureau, N., & Roussel, S. (2017). How do incidental emotions impact pro-environmental behavior? Evidence from the dictator game. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 66, 150-155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2016.04.003

  • Kai, Z., Woan, T. S., Jie, L., Goodale, E., Kitajima, K., Bagchi, R., & Harrison, R. D. (2014). Shifting baselines on a tropical forest frontier: Extirpations drive declines in local ecological knowledge. PLoS One, 9(3), Article e92931. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092931

  • Kates, R. W., Clark, W. C., Corell, R., Hall, J. M., Jaeger, C. C., Lowe, I., McCarthy, J. J., Schellnhuber, H. J., Bolin, B., Dickson, N. M., Faucheux, S., Gallopin, G. C., Grübler, A., Huntley, B., Jäger, J., Jodha, N. S., Kasperson, R. E., Mabogunje, A., Matson, P., . . .Svedin, U. (2001). Sustainability science. Science, 292(5517), 641-642. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1059386

  • Keith, R. J., Given, L. M., Martin, J. M., & Hochuli, D. F. (2021). Urban children’s connections to nature and environmental behaviors differ with age and gender. PLoS One, 16(7), Article e0255421. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0255421

  • Keitsch, M. M., & Vermeulen, W. J. V. (Eds.). (2021). Transdisciplinarity for sustainability: Aligning diverse practices. Routledge.

  • Keller, E., Marsh, J. E., Richardson, B. H., & Ball, L. J. (2022). A systematic review of the psychological distance of climate change: Towards the development of an evidence-based construct. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 81, Article 101822. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101822

  • Kellert, S. R., & Wilson, E. O. (1993). The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press.

  • Kidd, L. R., Bekessy, S. A., & Garrard, G. E. (2019). Neither hope nor fear: Empirical evidence should drive biodiversity conservation strategies. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 34(4), 278-282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2019.01.018

  • Kidd, L. R., Garrard, G. E., Bekessy, S. A., Mills, M., Camilleri, A. R., Fidler, F., Fielding, K. S., Gordon, A., Gregg, E. A., Kusmanoff, A. M., Louis, W., Moon, K., Robinson, J. A., Selinske, M. J., Shanahan, D., & Adams, V. M. (2019). Messaging matters: A systematic review of the conservation messaging literature. Biological Conservation, 236, 92-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2019.05.020

  • Kim, I., Hammond, M., & Milfont, T. L. (2023). Do environmental messages emphasising binding morals promote conservatives’ pro-environmentalism? A pre-registered replication. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, Article e8557. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.8557

  • Klebl, C., & Jetten, J. (2023). Perceived national wealth increases support for structural climate policies. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 91, Article 102055. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102055

  • Klein, J. T. (2020). Sustainability and collaboration: Crossdisciplinary and cross-sector horizons. Sustainability, 12(4), Article 1515. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12041515

  • Lamb, W. F., Mattioli, G., Levi, S., Roberts, J. T., Capstick, S., Creutzig, F., Minx, J. C., Müller-Hansen, F., Culhane, T., & Steinberger, J. K. (2020). Discourses of climate delay. Global Sustainability, 3, Article e17. https://doi.org/10.1017/sus.2020.13

  • Landon, A. C., Jacobs, M. H., Miller, C. A., Vaske, J. J., & Williams, B. D. (2020). Cognitive and affective predictors of Illinois residents’ perceived risks from gray wolves. Society & Natural Resources, 33(5), 574-593. https://doi.org/10.1080/08941920.2019.1664680

  • Lang, D. J., Wiek, A., Bergmann, M., Stauffacher, M., Martens, P., Moll, P., Swilling, M., & Thomas, C. J. (2012). Transdisciplinary research in sustainability science: Practice, principles, and challenges. Sustainability Science, 7, 25-43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-011-0149-x

  • Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K. H., Seifert, C. M., Schwarz, N., & Cook, J. (2012). Misinformation and its correction: Continued influence and successful debiasing. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 13(3), 106-131. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100612451018

  • Majid, A., Roberts, S. G., Cilissen, L., Emmorey, K., Nicodemus, B., O’Grady, L., Woll, B., LeLan, B., de Sousa, H., Cansler, B. L., Shayan, S., de Vos, C., Senft, G., Enfield, N. J., Razak, R. A., Fedden, S., Tufvesson, S., Dingemanse, M., Ozturk, O., . . .Levinson, S. C. (2018). Differential coding of perception in the world’s languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(45), 11369-11376. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1720419115

  • Manfredo, M. J., Bruskotter, J. T., Teel, T. L., Fulton, D., Schwartz, S. H., Arlinghaus, R., Oishi, S., Uskul, A. K., Redford, K., Kitayama, S., & Sullivan, L. (2017). Why social values cannot be changed for the sake of conservation. Conservation Biology, 31(4), 772-780. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.12855

  • Manfredo, M. J., Teel, T. L., & Dietsch, A. M. (2016). Implications of human value shift and persistence for biodiversity conservation. Conservation Biology, 30(2), 287-296. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.12619

  • Manfredo, M. J., Urquiza-Haas, E. G., Don Carlos, A. W., Bruskotter, J. T., & Dietsch, A. M. (2020). How anthropomorphism is changing the social context of modern wildlife conservation. Biological Conservation, 241, Article 108297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2019.108297

  • Martin, V. Y. (2020). Four common problems in environmental social research undertaken by natural scientists. Bioscience, 70(1), 13-16. https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biz128

  • Martin, L., White, M. P., Hunt, A., Richardson, M., Pahl, S., & Burt, J. (2020). Nature contact, nature connectedness and associations with health, wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviours. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 68, Article 101389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101389

  • McAfee, D., Doubleday, Z. A., Geiger, N., & Connell, S. D. (2019). Everyone loves a success story: Optimism inspires conservation engagement. Bioscience, 69(4), 274-281. https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biz019

  • Mikołajczak, K., Lees, A. C., Barlow, J., Sinclair, F., Trindade de Almeida, O., Souza, A. C., & Parry, L. (2021). Who knows, who cares? Untangling ecological knowledge and nature connection among Amazonian colonist farmers. People and Nature, 3(2), 431-445. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10183

  • Milfont, T. L. (2010). Global warming, climate change and human psychology. In V. Corral-Verdugo, C. H. García-Cadena & M. Frías-Arment (Eds.), Psychological approaches to sustainability: Current trends in theory, research and practice (pp. 19–42). Nova Science.

  • Milfont, T. L. (2021). The differential psychology of environmental protection/exploitation (La psicología diferencial de la protección/explotación medioambiental). PsyEcology, 12(3), 398-427. https://doi.org/10.1080/21711976.2021.1954394

  • Milfont, T. L., Duckitt, J., & Wagner, C. (2010). A cross-cultural test of the value–attitude–behavior hierarchy. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40(11), 2791-2813. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00681.x

  • Milfont, T. L., Harré, N., Sibley, C. G., & Duckitt, J. (2012). The climate-change dilemma: Examining the association between parental status and political party support. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42(10), 2386-2410. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00946.x

  • Milfont, T. L., & Markowitz, E. (2016). Sustainable consumer behavior: A multilevel perspective. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 112-117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.12.016

  • Milfont, T. L., Osborne, D., Yogeeswaran, K., & Sibley, C. G. (2020). The role of national identity in collective pro-environmental action. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 72, Article 101522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101522

  • Milfont, T. L., & Thomson, R. (2020). A within-country study of biased comparative judgements about the severity of environmental problems. Social Psychological Bulletin, 15(3), Article e3019. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.3019

  • Moore, F. C., Obradovich, N., Lehner, F., & Baylis, P. (2019). Rapidly declining remarkability of temperature anomalies may obscure public perception of climate change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(11), 4905-4910. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1816541116

  • Myers, T. A., Maibach, E. W., Roser-Renouf, C., Akerlof, K., & Leiserowitz, A. A. (2013). The relationship between personal experience and belief in the reality of global warming. Nature Climate Change, 3(4), 343-347. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1754

  • Newman, G., Wiggins, A., Crall, A., Graham, E., Newman, S., & Crowston, K. (2012). The future of citizen science: Emerging technologies and shifting paradigms. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment, 10(6), 298-304. https://doi.org/10.1890/110294

  • Newman, L., & Dale, A. (2013). Celebrating the mundane: Nature and the built environment. Environmental Values, 22(3), 401-413. https://doi.org/10.3197/096327113X13648087563827

  • Ng, S. T., Leung, A. K., & Chan, S. H. M. (2023). Through the lens of a naturalist: How learning about nature promotes nature connectedness via awe. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 92(1), Article 102069. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102069

  • Nilsson, D., Fielding, K., & Dean, A. J. (2020). Achieving conservation impact by shifting focus from human attitudes to behaviors. Conservation Biology, 34(1), 93-102. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13363

  • OECD. (2017). Behavioural insights and public policy: Lessons from around the world. OECD Publishing.

  • Oh, R. R. Y., Fielding, K. S., Carrasco, R. L., & Fuller, R. A. (2020). No evidence of an extinction of experience or emotional disconnect from nature in urban Singapore. People and Nature, 2(4), 1196-1209. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10148

  • Palmberg, I., Berg, I., Jeronen, E., Kärkkäinen, S., Norrgård-Sillanpää, P., Persson, C., Vilkonis, R., & Yli-Panula, E. (2015). Nordic–Baltic student teachers’ identification of and interest in plant and animal species: The importance of species identification and biodiversity for sustainable development. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 26(6), 549-571. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10972-015-9438-z

  • Papworth, S. K., Rist, J., Coad, L., & Milner-Gulland, E. J. (2009). Evidence for shifting baseline syndrome in conservation. Conservation Letters, 2(2), 93-100. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-263X.2009.00049.x

  • Pascual, U., Balvanera, P., Díaz, S., Pataki, G., Roth, E., Stenseke, M., Watson, R. T., Başak Dessane, E., Islar, M., Kelemen, E., Maris, V., Quaas, M., Subramanian, S. M., Wittmer, H., Adlan, A., Ahn, S., Al-Hafedh, Y. S., Amankwah, E., Asah, S. T., . . .Yagi, N. (2017). Valuing nature’s contributions to people: The IPBES approach. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability, 26–27, 7-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2016.12.006

  • Pateman, R., Dyke, A., & West, S. (2021). The diversity of participants in environmental citizen science. Citizen Science: Theory and Practice, 6(1), Article 9. https://doi.org/10.5334/cstp.369

  • Pinder, J., Fielding, K. S., & Fuller, R. A. (2020). Conservation concern among Australian undergraduates is associated with childhood socio-cultural experiences. People and Nature, 2(4), 1158-1171. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10145

  • Psychology of Change. (2023, October 19). Pepss Seminar #3: Why people do and don’t protect nature – Fielding, Van Eeden, Possingham, Milfont [Video]. YouTube. https://youtu.be/8fLPWyM8dqg?si=wEyk3ctwj4T_uvUz

  • Pyle, R. M. (2003). Nature matrix: Reconnecting people and nature. Oryx, 37(2), 206-214. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0030605303000383

  • Reddy, S. M. W., Montambault, J., Masuda, Y. J., Keenan, E., Butler, W., Fisher, J. R. B., Asah, S. T., & Gneezy, A. (2017). Advancing conservation by understanding and influencing human behaviour. Conservation Letters, 10(2), 248-256. https://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12252

  • Richardson, M., Hamlin, I., Elliott, L. R., & White, M. P. (2022). Country-level factors in a failing relationship with nature: Nature connectedness as a key metric for a sustainable future. Ambio, 51, 2201-2213. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13280-022-01744-w

  • Richardson, M., Passmore, H.-A., Barbett, L., Lumber, R., Thomas, R., & Hunt, A. (2020). The green care code: How nature connectedness and simple activities help explain pro-nature conservation behaviours. People and Nature, 2, 821-839. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10117

  • Richardson, M., & Sheffield, D. (2017). Three good things in nature: Noticing nearby nature brings sustained increases in connection with nature. PsyEcology, 8(1), 1-32. https://doi.org/10.1080/21711976.2016.1267136

  • Robinson, J. M., & Breed, M. F. (2019). Green prescriptions and their co-benefits: Integrative strategies for public and environmental health. Challenges, 10(1), Article 9. https://doi.org/10.1080/21711976.2016.1267136

  • Sangha, K. K., Russell-Smith, J., & Costanza, R. (2019). Mainstreaming indigenous and local communities’ connections with nature for policy decision-making. Global Ecology and Conservation, 19, Article e00668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gecco.2019.e00668

  • Schell, C. J., Dyson, K., Fuentes, T. L., Des Roches, S., Harris, N. C., Miller, D. S., Woelfle-Erskine, C. A., & Lambert, M. R. (2020). The ecological and evolutionary consequences of systemic racism in urban environments. Science, 369(6510), Article eaay4497. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aay4497

  • Schultz, P. W. (2011). Conservation means behavior. Conservation Biology, 25(6), 1080-1083. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1739.2011.01766.x

  • Schultz, P. W., Milfont, T. L., Chance, R. C., Tronu, G., Luís, S., Ando, K., Rasool, F., Roose, P. L., Ogunbode, C. A., Castro, J., & Gouveia, V. V. (2014). Cross-cultural evidence for spatial bias in beliefs about the severity of environmental problems. Environment and Behavior, 46(3), 267-302. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916512458579

  • Schultz, P. W., & Zelezny, L. C. (1998). Values and proenvironmental behavior: A five-country survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29(4), 540-558. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022198294003

  • Schultz, P. W., & Zelezny, L. (2003). Reframing environmental messages to be congruent with American values. Human Ecology Review, 10(2), 126-136.

  • Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 221–279). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60358-5

  • Sedawi, W., Ben Zvi Assaraf, O., & Reiss, M. J. (2021). Challenges in measuring “connectedness to nature” among indigenous children: Lessons from the Negev Bedouin. Cultural Studies of Science Education, 16, 193-229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11422-020-09995-3

  • Selinske, M. J., Garrard, G. E., Bekessy, S. A., Gordon, A., Kusmanoff, A. M., & Fidler, F. (2018). Revisiting the promise of conservation psychology. Conservation Biology, 32(6), 1464-1468. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13106

  • Selinske, M. J., Garrard, G. E., Gregg, E. A., Kusmanoff, A. M., Kidd, L. R., Cullen, M. T., Cooper, M., Geary, W. L., Hatty, M. A., Hames, F., Kneebone, S., McLeod, E. M., Ritchie, E. G., Squires, Z. E., Thomas, J., Willcock, M. A. W., Blair, S., & Bekessy, S. A. (2020). Identifying and prioritizing human behaviors that benefit biodiversity. Conservation Science and Practice, 2(9), Article e249. https://doi.org/10.1111/csp2.249

  • Shanahan, D. F., Lin, B. B., Gaston, K. J., Bush, R., & Fuller, R. A. (2014). Socio-economic inequalities in access to nature on public and private lands: A case study from Brisbane, Australia. Landscape and Urban Planning, 130, 14-23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.06.005

  • Shrum, T. R., Platt, N. S., Markowitz, E., & Syropoulos, S. (2023). A scoping review of the green parenthood effect on environmental and climate engagement. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 14(2), Article e818. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.818

  • Simaika, J. P., & Samways, M. J. (2010). Biophilia as a universal ethic for conserving biodiversity. Conservation Biology, 24(3), 903-906. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1739.2010.01485.x

  • Soga, M., & Gaston, K. J. (2016). Extinction of experience: The loss of human–nature interactions. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment, 14(2), 94-101. https://doi.org/10.1002/fee.1225

  • Soga, M., & Gaston, K. J. (2018). Shifting baseline syndrome: Causes, consequences, and implications. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment, 16(4), 222-230. https://doi.org/10.1002/fee.1794

  • Soga, M., & Gaston, K. J. (2023). Nature benefit hypothesis: Direct experiences of nature predict self-reported pro-biodiversity behaviors. Conservation Letters, 16(3), Article e12945. https://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12945

  • Song, J. Y., Klebl, C., & Bastian, B. (2023). Awe promotes moral expansiveness via the small-self. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, Article 1097627. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1097627

  • Soulé, M. E. (1985). What is conservation biology? Bioscience, 35(11), 727-734. https://doi.org/10.2307/1310054

  • Soutter, A. R. B., Bates, T. C., & Mõttus, R. (2020). Big Five and HEXACO personality traits, proenvironmental attitudes, and behaviors: A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 913-941. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620903019

  • Stanley, S. K., Milfont, T. L., Wilson, M. S., & Sibley, C. G. (2019). The influence of social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism on environmentalism: A five-year cross-lagged analysis. PLoS One, 14(7), Article e0219067. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219067

  • Stern, P. C. (2000). Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behaviour. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 407-424. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00175

  • Steven, R., Rakotopare, N., & Newsome, D. (2021). Avitourism tribes: As diverse as the birds they watch. In C. Pforr, R. Dowling & M. Volgger (Eds.), Consumer tribes in tourism: Contemporary perspectives on special-interest tourism (pp. 101–118). Springer Singapore.

  • Stoeckl, N., Jarvis, D., Larson, S., Larson, A., Grainger, D., & Ewamian Aboriginal Corporation. (2021). Australian Indigenous insights into ecosystem services: Beyond services towards connectedness — People, place and time. Ecosystem Services, 50, Article 101341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2021.101341

  • Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations. Academic Press.

  • Tajfel, H. (1982). Social identity and intergoup relations. Cambridge University Press.

  • Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Brooks/Cole.

  • Taylor, D. E. (2018). Racial and ethnic differences in connectedness to nature and landscape preferences among college students. Environmental Justice, 11(3), 118-136. https://doi.org/10.1089/env.2017.0040

  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440-463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963

  • Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Wakslak, C. (2007). Construal levels and psychological distance: Effects on representation, prediction, evaluation, and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17(2), 83-95. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1057-7408(07)70013-X

  • Uzzell, D. L. (2000). The pscyho-spatial dimensions of global environmental problems. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 20(4), 307-318. https://doi.org/10.1006/jevp.2000.0175

  • Veríssimo, D., Macmillan, D. C., Smith, R. J., Crees, J., & Davies, Z. G. (2014). Has climate change taken prominence over biodiversity conservation? Bioscience, 64(7), 625-629. https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biu079

  • von Döhren, P., & Haase, D. (2015). Ecosystem disservices research: A review of the state of the art with a focus on cities. Ecological Indicators, 52, 490-497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolind.2014.12.027

  • Wang, L., Sheng, G., She, S., & Xu, J. (2023). Impact of empathy with nature on pro-environmental behaviour. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 47(2), 652-668. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijcs.12856

  • Whitburn, J., Linklater, W., & Abrahamse, W. (2020). Meta-analysis of human connection to nature and proenvironmental behavior. Conservation Biology, 34(1), 180-193. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13381

  • Wolch, J. R., Byrne, J., & Newell, J. P. (2014). Urban green space, public health, and environmental justice: The challenge of making cities ‘just green enough’. Landscape and Urban Planning, 125, 234-244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.01.017

  • Wolsko, C., Ariceaga, H., & Seiden, J. (2016). Red, white, and blue enough to be green: Effects of moral framing on climate change attitudes and conservation behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 7-19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.02.005

  • Yang, Y., Hu, J., Jing, F., & Nguyen, B. (2018). From awe to ecological behavior: The mediating role of connectedness to nature. Sustainability, 10(7), Article 2477. https://doi.org/10.3390/su10072477

  • Zelezny, L. C., Chua, P.-P., & Aldrich, C. (2000). New ways of thinking about environmentalism: Elaborating on gender differences in environmentalism. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 443-457. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00177